Skip to main content
Log in

A Generative System for Intentional Action?

  • Published:
Topoi Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been proposed that intentional actions are supplied by a generative system of the sort described by Chomsky for language. In this paper I aim to provide a closer analysis of this claim for the sake of conceptual clarification. To this end, I will first clarify what is involved in the thesis of a structural analogy between language and action, and then I will consider what kind of evidence there seems to be in favour of the thesis of a neurobiological identity. On this basis, I will subsequently focus on two definitional issues. The first is whether, as the claim of a generative system for intentional action suggests, humans may perform an infinite number of possible actions. The second is whether, as the claim of a generative system for intentional action suggests, what is at issue is conscious planning of action and therefore controlled processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I am not considering here the way in which, even for the same utterance, communicative intention may change as a function of context. However, at a first approximation that issue may be set aside. As a matter of fact, in the quotation above Sperber and Wilson (2002) seem to suggest that communicative intentions are just as complex as it would be predicted by the syntactic structure of the corresponding utterances.

References

  • Baars B, Gage N (2010) Cognition, brain and consciousness. Academic Press, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin D, Baird J (2001) Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends Cogn Sci 5:171–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer P (1995) Recalling past events: from infancy to early childhood. Ann Child Dev 11:25–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Bookheimer S (2002) Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 25:151–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick M (2008) Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends Cogn Sci 12:201–208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clerget E, Winderickx A, Fadiga L et al (2009) Role of Broca’s area in encoding sequential human actions: a virtual lesion study. NeuroReport 20:1496–1499

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga L, Craighero L, D’Ausilio A (2009) Broca’s area in language, action, and music. Ann NY Acad Sci 1169:448–458

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio P, Cantagallo A, Craighero L et al (2009) Encoding of human action in Broca’s area. Brain 132:1980–1988

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster J (1990) Prefrontal cortex and the bridging of temporal gaps in the perception-action cycle. Ann NY Acad Sci 608:318–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster J (2001) The prefrontal cortex-An update: time is of the essence. Neuron 2:319–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster J (2003) Cortex and mind. Unifying cognition, Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuster J (2004) Upper processing stages of the perception-action cycle. Trends Cogn Sci 8:143–145

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough P, Nobre A, Devlin J (2005) Dissociating linguistic processes in the left inferior frontal cortex with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci 25:8010–8016

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafton S, Hamilton A (2007) Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. Hum Movement Sci 26:590–616

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort P (2005) On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci 9:416–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higuchi S, Chaminade T, Imamizu H et al (2007) Shared neural correlates for language and tool use in Broca’s area. NeuroReport 20:1376–1381

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (2002) Foundations of language. Brain, meaning, grammar, Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (2007a) A parallel architecture perspective on language processing. Brain Res 1146:2–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (2007b) Language, consciousness, culture: essays on mental structure (Jean Nicod Lectures). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R, Pinker S (2005) The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, & Chomsky). Cognition 97:211–225

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan J (2003) Consciousness on the edge: The intentional nature of experience. In: Science and Consciousness Review. http://www.scicon.org/news/articles/20040101.html. Cited 19 Feb 2013

  • Koechlin E, Jubault T (2006) Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron 50:963–974

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperberg G (2007) Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: challenges to syntax. Brain Res 1146:23–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt W (1989) Speaking: from intention to articulation. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzone M (2014) Automatic and controlled processes in pragmatics. In: Capone A, Lo Piparo F, Carapezza M (ed) Perspectives on linguistic pragmatics. Springer (forthcoming)

  • Mazzone M, Campisi E (2013) Distributed intentionality: a model of intentional behavior in humans. Philos Psychol 26:267–290

    Google Scholar 

  • Pastra K, Aloimonos Y (2012) The minimalist grammar of action. Philos T R Soc B 367:103–117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roby-Brami A, Hermsdorfer J, Roy A et al (2012) A neuropsychological perspective on the link between language and praxis in modern humans. Philos T R Soc B 367:144–160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rougier N, Noelle D, Braver T et al (2005) Prefrontal cortex and flexible cognitive control: rules without symbols. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:7338–7343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sitnikova T, Holcomb P, Kiyonaga K et al (2008) Two neurocognitive mechanisms of semantic integration during the comprehension of visual real-world events. J Cogn Neurosci 20:1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (2002) Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind Lang 17:3–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti M, Weniger D (2006) Broca’s area: a supramodal hierarchical processor? Cortex 42:491–494

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A, Flynn E, Brown K et al (2006) Imitation of hierarchical action structure by young children. Dev Sci 9:574–582

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marco Mazzone.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mazzone, M. A Generative System for Intentional Action?. Topoi 33, 77–85 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-013-9186-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-013-9186-7

Keywords

Navigation