A unifying aim for developmental psychology is to understand the processes of change in human behavior (Adolph & Robinson, 2008; Kagan, 2008; Muller & Giesbrecht, 2008). Theories of development are often contrasted in terms of their accounts of change, in particular whether processes of change are qualitative or quantitative, and continuous or discontinuous. Importantly, methodological tools often influence theoretical accounts of change by yielding data that favors one account of change over another (McCall, 1981). In the present article, we will briefly review the major methodological tools in developmental psychology and then introduce a new method—a continuous unified electronic diary method for gathering developmental data.

Experiments are systematic and rigorous methods for studying elicited behavior, typically in a laboratory setting. Because they involve the manipulation of an independent variable, experimental methods allow clear analysis of cause–effect relations and are useful for addressing questions about the relation between a specific behavioral phenomenon and a specific eliciting context (Cronbach, 1957; Danziger, 2000; Holmes & Teti, 2008). Experimental methods are not suitable, however, for addressing questions about the spontaneous emergence of behavioral phenomena, and are of limited usefulness for addressing questions about behavior across time and context, or, in other words, behavioral change (Stone, Broderick, Kaell, DelesPaul, & Porter, 2000). In addition, experiments are susceptible to data distortion caused by low sampling frequency (Adolph, Robinson, Young, & Gill-Alvarez, 2008; Kuhn, 1995). As a result, experiments may miss periods of instability and change, leading to inaccurate estimates of developmental trajectories. These problems are especially critical for low-frequency and/or newly emerging behaviors. For these reasons, experimental methods have limited utility for addressing questions about processes of change.

Observations are a practical method for studying naturally occurring behavior, whether spontaneous or elicited, and whether in the laboratory, home, or daycare environment. Observational methods allow analysis of relations between children and the contexts in which they are developing and are useful for addressing questions about the bidirectional influences that parents and children have on one another (Bakeman, 1983; Bornstein, 1985; Danis, Bourdais, & Ruel, 2000; Mitchell, 1979). Observational methods deliver greater contextual information and, as a result, can have greater generalizability than experimental methods. Observational methods are labor intensive, however, and are prone to the same problems of sampling frequency as experimental methods, with the result that they too have limited utility for addressing questions about processes of change.

Parent reports are a form of observation that rely on parents as observers and reporters of behavior, according to their day-to-day experience with a child. The most common forms of parent report are checklists and questionnaires (Dekker, Nunn, Einfeld, Tonge, & Koot, 2002; Dunn, 1990; Fenson et al., 2000; Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Reznick & Goldfield, 1992). Because they draw on parents’ regular experience with their children, checklists and questionnaires are less susceptible to some of the problems of sampling frequency associated with experimental and observational methods. Because parent-report methods are usually retrospective, however, they are susceptible to other sources of data distortion (Seifer, 2005), in particular parents’ abilities to recall behaviors that have occurred in the past (Green, Rafaeli, Bolger, Shrout, & Reis, 2006; Reznick & Goldfield, 1994). Importantly, most studies using checklists and questionnaires have limited options for assessing parents’ compliance or reliability as reporters. In addition, because checklists and questionnaires are administered at a specific moment in time, they are of limited utility for identifying emergence or change in behaviors. Most checklists and questionnaires also lack critical contextual information such as social and physical context, which in turn limits their utility for addressing questions about processes of change in development.

Methodologically, parent-report diaries address many of the limitations of the experimental and observational methods discussed above. Because parent-report diaries are based on continuous observation, they provide a high sampling rate over a specified period, and, in addition, they offer data about children in their natural environment (e.g., Callanan & Oakes, 1992; Johnson & Mervis, 1997; Naigles, Hoff, & Vear, 2009; Nelson, 1989; Pine, 1995; Robinson & Mervis, 1999; Tomasello, 1992; Wolfson, Lacks, & Futterman, 1992). Even the most rigorous schedule of experimental testing can miss critical points in development because of sampling distortion (Adolph et al., 2008; Lewis, 2000). By comparison, the event-based data from diaries reveal patterns of behavior and development with less sampling skew and distortion (Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003). Parent-report diaries thus have the potential to yield extremely valuable developmental data, including information on age of emergence, age of skill progression, and contextual variables such as precursor variables, eliciting factors, and rewards.

Diary studies have, however, historically lacked the rigor and systematic strengths of experimental methods. In particular, previous studies involving parent-report diaries have frequently had very small sample sizes, high attrition, and lacked sufficient assessment of compliance and reliability (Stone, Shiffman, Schwartz, Broderick, & Hufford 2002; Miller, 1981; Naigles et al., 2009). Until recently, the only method for collecting parent-report diaries was paper and pencil, with the result that reporting was an onerous task, and it was not possible to monitor when entries were made or to assess compliance with reporting instructions, such as entering reports at requested times. Reliability of reporters has also been a concern with diary methods. Low reliability may result from insufficient training of reporters to observe and record behaviors; from the onerous nature of paper-and-pencil diaries, which may lead to fewer and/or more retrospective reports, or simply from parental biases in reporting.

Over the past decade, psychologists and social scientists have increasingly turned to electronic diaries to address these problems while at the same time exploiting the many advantages of diary data (Green et al., 2006). Electronic diaries allow for easier and more consistent reports across participants (Hufford & Shields, 2002). For example, in one study comparing paper and electronic diary reports of infant fussiness and caregiver holding, maternal reporters described paper diaries as more onerous and made more frequent entries with electronic diaries (Lam et al., 2010). In addition, because electronic diaries contain automatic time-stamp information for entries, they allow researchers to monitor and assess participant entries for compliance and reliability (Ebner-Priemer & Trull, 2009). Electronic diaries, sometimes referred to as ecological momentary assessment or ambulatory assessment, are now increasingly used by researchers investigating psychological phenomena such as emotional functioning, mood disorders, stress, organizational behavior, and health treatments (Alpers, 2009; Gunthert, Conner, Armeli, Tennen, Covault, & Kranzler, 2007; Ebner-Priemer et al., 2007; Ebner-Priemer, & Sawitzki, 2007; Fahrenberg, Myrtek, Pawlik, & Perrez, 2007; Myrtek, Aschenbrenner, & Brugner 2005; Hoppmann & Riediger, 2009; Jamison et al., 2001; King, Lluch, Stubbs, & Blundell, 1997; Klumb, Elfering, & Herre, 2009).

In the present article, we introduce a new event-based adaptation of electronic diaries intended for use by developmental psychologists. Electronic diaries are a potentially valuable methodological tool for developmental psychologists because they draw on continuous observation and allow the study of behavior in natural contexts, while at the same time offering consistency and ease of use across reporters, as well as assessment of compliance and reliability. Most studies using electronic diary data collection have involved self-report, however, whereas collecting data on infants and children requires another person, usually a parent, to do the reporting.

The CUE diary method has two components. First, we developed a reporting method suitable for parent reporters that yields data containing sufficient contextual information to be useful to developmental psychologists interested in change processes. We used the Experience Sampling Programme (ESP.4) (Feldman Barrett & Barrett, 2001) to develop a series of event-based questions that allowed parents to identify and record target behaviors on a continuous basis. The questions were designed to elicit accurate behavioral observations with appropriate contextual information, and at the same time to ensure ease of use and consistency of reporting across parents. To do so, we used a combination of open-ended questions with free-text response fields and closed-class questions with categorical answers available from a drop-down menu. The complete CUE diary method addresses the development of communicative, imitative, and motor behaviors, but in the present report, we focus on motor behaviors only, since they were critical to our assessment of the method (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Questions on motor development used in continuous unified electronic (CUE) diary method. Greeting and exit messages are shown in grey. Questions had either free-text responses (dashed line) or menu responses (solid line)

Second, we developed a training and support system to ensure that parent reporters knew how to use the electronic diary, had sufficient understanding of the target behaviors to be able to identify them accurately, and were able to remember what was requested of them. This was an important step in adapting electronic diary methods to the study of human development, since electronic diaries have previously been used primarily for self-report rather than the reporting of parent-observed infant and child behaviors. To provide training and support for parent-observed recording of developmental phenomena, we recruited mothers into the study during pregnancy, trained them to identify and record infant behaviors, and gave them supporting materials to take home. Mothers were given clear definitions of target behaviors and were instructed to record the first three occurrences of each behavior as soon as possible after it was observed. Mothers were asked to identify and record target developmental phenomena from birth to 18 months. To help maintain accuracy and motivation throughout that period, maternal reporters also attended monthly assessment meetings and were given monthly feedback on a separate occasion.

To test the utility of the CUE diary method, we evaluated compliance, reliability, and validity of maternal reporters. To evaluate compliance, we examined reporting of three target motor behaviors: palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking. We chose these three behaviors because they span the complexity of motor development over infancy, where the palmar grasp is a reflex, pincer grip is a fine motor behavior, and walking is a gross motor behavior. Compliance evaluation focused on whether behaviors were accurately identified (or, in other words, were consistent with the provided definition), had been recorded at least twice, and had been recorded as soon as possible after observation.

To evaluate the reliability of the CUE diary method as well as the reliability of individual reporters, we compared CUE diary data to researcher assessments of the three target motor behaviors (palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking). Reliability evaluation focused on whether maternal reporters identified a behavior at least as soon as a researcher identified it, and not more than two months before.

Having developed an event-based electronic diary method for the study of infant and child development, we then wanted to demonstrate the value of the data yielded by this method. To do so, we first analyzed data about onset for the seven motor behaviors of palmar grasp, rolls from side to back, reaches when sitting, pincer grip, crawling, walking, and climbing stairs. These behaviors span the first 18 months of life, encompassing reflex, fine, and gross motor development. Secondly, we compared age-of-emergence for our three target motor behaviors of palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking, from CUE diary data and researcher-assessed data, to examine whether electronic diary data would be less prone to data distortion. Because reflex and fine motor behaviors involve fewer components, we expected it would be easier for maternal reporters to identify such behaviors accurately. By comparison, gross motor behaviors involve multiple components, and we therefore expected that it would be more difficult for maternal reporters to identify them accurately. We also expected that the continuous, event-based sampling of the CUE diary method would yield estimations of developmental trajectories without the sampling distortion of researcher-assessed data.

The present study is the first step of a larger study using the CUE diary method to investigate interactions among cognitive, communicative, and motor development. Evaluating compliance and reliability was critical to our study because doing so not only allows us to establish the validity of the method, but also to identify and exclude unreliable reporters at subsequent steps in the larger study.

Method

Participants

Forty expectant mothers were recruited during their last trimester of pregnancy to participate in First Steps, a longitudinal study of infant development from birth until 18 months. Expecting parents were recruited from community organizations within Cardiff. The First Steps design involved continuous electronic diary reporting from birth to 18 months, and monthly testing from 2 until 18 months. At each monthly testing session, families were given £25 in shopping vouchers and a baby gift, such as a toy, t-shirt, or book, in return for their participation. As an incentive to complete the study, families were given an additional £250 in shopping vouchers at the end of the study. The sample of reporters ranged in age, nationality, ethnicity, education level, socioeconomic status, marital status, and the number of previous children, as shown in Table 1. One mother did not begin the study. Of the remaining 39 mother–infant dyads, all infants were born to term with 18 females and 21 males.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of maternal reporters, presented as a percent of each group

The Cardiff University School of Psychology Research Ethics Committee and the South East Wales Local Research Ethics Committee of the Cardiff and Vale National Health Service Trust both reviewed the study protocol and granted ethical approval. Each maternal reporter gave informed consent prior to participation in the study.

Electronic diary

Palm Pilot Z20s were installed with Version 4 of the Experience Sampling Programme (ESP.4) (Feldman Barrett & Barrett, 2001). ESP.4 is free, open-source software for collecting questionnaire data electronically. The software presents questions and records answers and response times (RTs) and creates an automatic time stamp for each entry. Once a question is completed, the software proceeds to the next question automatically. To prevent participants from changing entries post hoc, the software does not allow reviewing or changing of entries. For analysis, data from individual handheld devices can be uploaded onto a central computer.

A series of event-based questions were created in ESP that allowed parents to identify and record communicative, imitative, and motor behaviors on a continuous basis (see Fig. 1). Because our assessment of the reliability and validity of the method focused on motor behaviors, only motor behaviors are reported here. Some questions were open-ended and had free-text response fields, whereas other questions were closed class and had categorical answers available from a drop-down menu. Each entry thus identified a particular infant behavior and various contextual variables, including the social setting, the physical setting, whether the behavior was elicited or spontaneous, and whether this was the first, second, or third occasion on which the behavior occurred. Sampling was event based and therefore determined by the frequency of infant behavior, rather than in response to a time-based prompt from the handheld device.

Procedure

Initial training and support material

During the last trimester of pregnancy, maternal reporters participated in an introductory training session. During the session, a researcher demonstrated the CUE diary method, asked the mother to create practice entries, and gave the mother a study notebook with supporting materials (see Appendices A and B for examples). An important component of the study notebook was the first What to Expect (WTE) sheet (see Appendix C for an abbreviated example). Each WTE sheet provided a brief summary of infant development during the specified period and identified target behaviors for maternal reporters to observe and record. WTE sheets with new target behaviors were provided in 3-month intervals so that mothers did not need to hold a large number of target behaviors in mind at one time. Further training was provided with each WTE sheet. At the end of the training session, maternal reporters were instructed to create further practice entries at home.

Maternal reporters were instructed to begin reporting from the birth of their infant, and to enter each target behavior as soon as possible after it occurred. ESP creates an automatic time-stamp for each entry, including both date and time. If mothers reported an event that had occurred on a previous day, they had to enter the date it had occurred. This allowed us to evaluate entries on the basis of compliance with this reporting requirement. In some instances, mothers kept notes in their personal diaries and created ESP entries on the basis of these notes. To ensure a clear threshold, mothers were told that recording any event more than 2 weeks after it occurred would invalidate the entry.

Monthly assessments

All maternal reporters and their infants attended monthly assessments. These assessments were organized into groups, with infants allocated into a group according to the week of the month that he or she was born. This ensured that monthly visits occurred close to infants’ monthly birthdays.

At each monthly assessment, diary entries were uploaded onto a researcher computer, and after the visit, entries were transferred to an online database. At this time, mothers were asked whether they had any problems with identifying or recording behaviors, or needed any clarification. At the 4-, 6-, 9-, 12-, and 15-month visits, a new WTE sheet was given to the mother, together with further training on identifying target behaviors.

At each monthly assessment, infants were also tested by a trained researcher. This testing included researcher-assessed motor milestones that formed the basis of our reliability evaluation, and other experimental measures (not reported here). As in previous parent-report diary studies, such as Bodnarchuk and Boudreau (2004), we retrospectively validated the CUE diary method and the reliability of maternal reporters by comparing the age-of-emergence of target behaviors as recorded by the maternal reporter with the age-of-emergence found in researcher assessments. These researcher assessments followed World Health Organization (WHO) methods for assessing motor development (Wijnhoven et al., 2004).

Further support and training

Each maternal reporter was assigned a key researcher who was present at the introductory training session and maintained regular contact with the mother throughout the study. Following each monthly visit, key researchers reviewed the entries of maternal reporters and created a feedback sheet identifying the target behaviors for which the maternal reporter had created valid entries in the previous month and the target behaviors that the maternal reporter needed to watch for over the coming month. This monitoring and feedback system was designed to increase ease of observation for participants and to maximize reporting compliance.

Results

Compliance assessment

For an entry to be considered valid, it had to contain sufficient detail to match the target behavior definitions given in the WTE sheets (see Appendix C for an example). Entries were considered invalid if they were recorded before distribution of the WTE sheet that described the behavior, or if they were recorded more than 2 weeks after occurrence. This delay was evaluated by comparing the time stamp of each entry with the reported date of occurrence. Entries that failed to meet these criteria were excluded from analysis. Such entries made up less than 1 % of the total number of entries analyzed.

Two maternal reporters failed to complete the study, and their data was excluded on this basis. One infant was referred for developmental delay and was excluded on this basis, since the instructions and training procedures were designed and piloted with typically developing infants. One maternal reporter failed to comply with the requirement of having two or more valid entries for palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking and was excluded from the sample.

Reliability assessment

To assess reliability of the method as well as reliability of individual maternal reporters, maternal reports were compared with researcher assessments for three target motor behaviors (palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking). For any data from a maternal reporter to be included in the final CUE diary data set, she was required to be reliable for all three of the target motor behaviors. A maternal reporter’s reliability for each behavior was evaluated according to the following criteria: Each behavior must have been recorded by the maternal reporter at least by the date on which a researcher assessment identified the behavior, and each behavior must have been recorded by the maternal reporter not more than two months before the researcher assessment identified the behavior. Following the reasoning of the WHO study (Wijnhoven et al., 2004), we reasoned that because mothers have more opportunities to observe behavior, they would identify behaviors as soon as or before researchers could do so, but that we needed to have a reasonable threshold for the researcher to identify the behavior. We chose two consecutive months of testing as this threshold, reasoning that if an infant was capable of a behavior, he or she might not evidence the behavior during the first assessment, but should be able to demonstrate the behavior during at least one of two assessments. This somewhat conservative threshold was designed to ensure maximum reliability across the dataset, and in so doing to increase confidence in future analyses of CUE diary data. Seven maternal reporters failed the described reliability criteria and were excluded from the final sample of CUE data as a result. After the application of compliance and reliability criteria, 81 % of maternal reporters remained. Table 1 presents demographic information for maternal reporters grouped according to inclusion status. Maternal reporters whose data were included or excluded in the final sample had similar ages, levels of education, and number of children.

Means and other descriptives

The mean age and standard deviation for the CUE diary motor behaviors of palmar grasp, rolls from side to back, reaches when sitting, pincer grip, crawling, walking, and climbing stairs are shown in Table 2. The first column denotes the full sample before exclusion criteria were applied. The second column contains information from maternal reporters excluded on the basis of compliance and reliability criteria. The third column shows information from the final CUE sample, and the final column shows data from researcher-administered assessments. The range of age of emergence was quite small for the palmar grasp, a reflex behavior observed shortly after birth. By comparison, the range of age of emergence was larger for the pincer grip, crawling, and walking.

Table 2 Age of emergence for motor behaviors presented as mean +/− SD

Validity assessments

Finally, we evaluated the CUE diary method by comparing age of emergence from CUE data for the final sample with age of emergence from researcher assessment (Columns 3 and 4 in Table 2). Researcher assessments consistently yielded higher age-of-emergence data and reduced within-group variation.

Figure 2 demonstrates the range of onset ages for all seven motor behaviors. Periods of overlap between certain behaviors, such as reaching when sitting and pincer grip, are evident. In one case, such overlap suggests reporting errors: The earliest report of climbing stairs preceded the earliest report of walking entry, a contradiction that suggests climbing stairs was not reported accurately.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Range of ages of onset for motor behaviors reported in continuous unified electronic (CUE) diary method, indicating overlap for periods of motor skill emergence. Palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking validated via researcher-administered assessment

Figure 3 shows a comparison between the distributions for age of emergence yielded by each method. The CUE diary data consistently produced earlier data on infants meeting milestones.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Histograms demonstrating distortion in age of emergence from researcher assessments compared to continuous unified electronic (CUE) diary method data, with lines of best fit

Finally, example CUE diary entries for each behavior are shown in Table 3. Within the example it is interesting to note the contextual information provided for the fine and gross motor entries.

Table 3 Example entries for each of the target motor behaviors

Discussion

We designed and evaluated an event-based, electronic maternal reporting method that allows real-time recording of infant and child behavior in natural contexts. Event-based electronic diary methods allow parents to create entries based on continuous observation, increase consistency across different entries and reporters, and allow for assessment of compliance and reliability.

Designing a continuous unified electronic diary method

Our primary aim was to design an event-based electronic diary reporting method suitable for the study of infant and child development. Diary data is valuable for the study of development because it draws on continuous observation and because it allows researchers to draw on rich contextual information. New parents have many demands on their time, and electronic diaries have previously been shown to offer greater ease of use than paper and pencil diaries (Lam et al., 2010). As a result, electronic diaries as a method encourage compliance and continued participation (Stone et al., 2002).

The CUE diary method allows researchers to gather contextual information using a combination of open-ended and closed-class questions. Understanding the role of context in shaping behavior is critical to understanding human development, and previous studies have shown that emergent behaviors are influenced by both physical and social environments (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000). The CUE diary method can be used to identify age of emergence as well as the physical and social contexts in which behaviors emerge.

The CUE diary method is suitable for studying development across a range of domains. In the present study, we focused on motor behaviors because of their reliable elicitation once present in the infant’s behavioral repertoire, and because we wanted to assess the reliability and value of the CUE diary method by comparing diary data with researcher assessments. Our complete CUE diary method included questions on cognitive and communicative development. The CUE diary method thus allows for the unified study of development across domains. Future research from our data set, for example, will examine the relations between motor and communicative development.

We used ESP software because it offered an easy-to-use platform with sufficient flexibility for our study, and installed the software on Palm Pilots. The CUE diary method is independent of the platform, however, and could easily be adapted for more recent versions of handheld computers, smartphones, or tablets.

Training maternal reporters

Previous electronic diary studies have, for the most part, involved self-report. Those studies that have involved parental reporting of infant behavior have focused on crying and sleeping, states that are both easily identified (Lam et al., 2010). To ensure that maternal reporters could remember and identify specific behaviors correctly, we developed a training and support system to accompany the electronic diaries. Future studies might implement these training components electronically, by using video training and other electronic resources.

Because the overall study involved comparisons across experimental and naturalistic data, the study design also included monthly visits to the university laboratory. Diary data was uploaded during these monthly meetings, and any immediate problems with the diaries were addressed. This regular contact may have had a positive effect on continued participation and compliance, but in future studies many of the conditions could be met by a combination of internet-based data collection and personal contact, which could include telephone conversations as well as electronic chats.

Feedback was also critical to the training and support system. Because the ESP software locks an entry as soon as it is completed, maternal reporters were not able to review previous questions or previous entries. To ensure that mothers knew which behaviors to watch for at any point in time, and to limit their dependence on memory for previous entries, entry feedback was given to mothers on a monthly basis. Following each upload of diary data, individual reports were created by researchers and sent to each maternal reporter. The reports summarized the past month’s entries and gave a list of the yet-to-be-recorded target behaviors. Although other software could be configured to generate reports automatically, our researcher-led reports had the benefit of allowing researchers to screen for any incorrect entries and then reiterate behavioral definitions to reporters.

For future studies, the CUE Diary Method could be adapted for other designs with reduced training and support and larger sample sizes. Our training and support systems for maternal reporters required a substantial investment of researcher time. Further studies are needed to address whether similarly high levels of reliability could be achieved with reduced training and support.

Assessing maternal reporter compliance

Compliance evaluation focused on reporting of three motor behaviors: a reflex, a fine motor behavior, and a gross motor behavior. Our evaluation criteria concerned the timing of the entry, the quality of the entry, and the number of observations.

An important disadvantage of traditional paper-and-pencil diaries is the impossibility of monitoring when entries are made, and in particular whether participants comply with time-related reporting instructions. We asked maternal reporters to create an entry for each target behavior as soon as possible after it had been observed. Because the CUE diary method included automatic time stamping of each entry, we were able to compare the reported date of an observed behavior with the actual entry date, and to disallow entries that were outside of an acceptable temporal threshold. Future studies could automate this evaluation procedure to reduce researcher time.

To further assess compliance, trained researchers reviewed each entry, checking whether the entry contained sufficient detail to identify the behavior, whether the entry met the definition provided for the behavior, and how many entries had been recorded for each behavior. The electronic format of the data facilitated this process by providing researchers with easy access to the entries in a spreadsheet format. We would not, however, recommend automating this checking procedure. The expert judgment of trained researchers was critical to this aspect of our evaluation, and provided the basis for feedback and further training. In future studies, this aspect of compliance evaluation could be handled through internet-based communications as well as telephone conversations.

Assessing maternal reporter reliability

Reliability was evaluated by comparing CUE diary data with researcher assessments for three target behaviors. Our evaluation criteria focused on whether maternal reporters identified behaviors at least as soon as a researcher identified them, and not more than two-months before that.

Our evaluation criteria were relatively strict. We required that each maternal reporter recorded each of our three motor milestones within this two-month window for any of her data to be included in future analyses. Our motivation for this stringent assessment method was that our overall data set also included other behaviors that we did not compare against researcher assessment. In order to be more confident about the reliability of those data, we applied a no-fails rule to the data where we did have researcher assessments. Over 80 % of maternal reporters were deemed reliable, indicating that the CUE diary method is a valid and reliable method for collecting data on infant development.

Future studies using the CUE diary method could adopt one of two strategies for assessing reliability. A technology-based strategy would be to ask parents to provide documentary evidence of a select number of behaviors, in the form of photographs or videos. Researchers could then compare entries against documentary evidence to assess reliability. Because we used handheld devices that did not contain cameras, this option was not available to us, but because of recent increases in the availability, quality and affordability of smartphones with cameras, this should be a good option for the future.

A second strategy for assessing reliability in future studies would be to combine CUE diary data with researcher assessments, as we did, but to require fewer researcher assessments. In our present study, the majority of participants who failed reliability criteria for one target behavior also failed the reliability criteria for other target behaviors, suggesting that future studies adopting this strategy could use fewer researcher assessments to identify unreliable reporters. This strategy seems particularly useful for mixed-method studies in which the overall study design might require laboratory testing for other purposes.

Advantages of electronic diaries when examining developmental change

Our final aim was to compare CUE diary data with researcher assessments to establish the value of the CUE diary method. We were particularly interested in whether the CUE diary method produced a more accurate and detailed account of development. We found that the CUE diary method yielded greater detail of contextual information, earlier ages of emergence across motor behaviors, and a finer depiction of individual differences.

Studying the shape of developmental change requires an accurate tool for gathering information on the emergence of target behaviors. The CUE diary method combines the richness and flexibility of parent-report diary data with a diary method approximating the systematicity and rigor of experimental methods, and allows testing of causal hypotheses about human development. With appropriate piloting, the CUE diary method could also be adapted for the study of atypical development, including developmental delay. For example, the CUE diary method would be suitable for recording motor behaviors of infants whose motor development appears to be delayed, if training materials and support were designed for such samples. The contextual information contained in the CUE diary method, as well as the unified approach to studying development across domains, may be particularly useful for examining questions about developmental delay.

Limitations of electronic diaries for developmental studies

At present, electronic diary studies are expensive as compared with other forms of parent report, including checklists, questionnaires, and paper-and-pencil diaries. Because smartphones were not widely available when our study began, we provided every participant with a handheld computer, at a substantial cost per participant. In addition, our study procedures included extensive support for maternal reporters, and this support was time consuming, and therefore expensive. Importantly, however, the relative cost of electronic diary studies is likely to change quickly as technology changes. Future studies could adapt the CUE diary method for smartphones and might even rely on participant-owned devices. Future studies might also use electronic resources for training and feedback to reduce costs. For example, video training and electronic messaging could be used for some of the procedures that involved one-on-one staff time in our study.

In our present study, some motor behaviors appeared easier for maternal reporters to accurately identify and record than others. For example, comparison of the earliest entries for walking and climbing stairs suggests that some maternal reporters may have been recording climbing stairs inaccurately, for instance, recording a scaffolded version of climbing stairs, rather than independent, upright climbing. In comparison, CUE data for all six of the remaining motor behaviors showed emergence within the range expected, and in the order of emergence expected. Similarly, the three behaviors of palmar grasp, pincer grip, and walking, all showed high levels of reliability, as predicted. Future studies would need to include piloting to ensure clear definitions which support accurate reporting of complex behaviors.

Conclusions

The CUE diary method is the first adaptation of event-based electronic diary methods for the study of infant and child development. Like historical parent-report diaries, the CUE diary method is based on continuous observation and provides data about infants in their natural environment. As a result, the CUE diary method allows researchers to collect data on age of emergence and important contextual variables such as precursor variables, eliciting factors, and rewards. Unlike traditional paper-and-pencil diaries, the CUE diary method allows for larger samples and reduced attrition because it is easier and quicker for participants to use due to menus, data uploading capabilities, and other time-saving features. In addition, it facilitates assessment of compliance and reliability. Through its adaptable design, the CUE diary method may be applied to other hand-held devices such as smartphones in the future.

Kuhn (1995) argued that developmental psychology aims to see the process of change, rather than simply its products. The CUE diary method is an excellent methodological tool for investigating the mechanisms of change in human development and yields event-driven, context-rich, longitudinal data across development. The CUE diary method opens the door to examining the dynamic nature of human development.