Background

During the past decades, device miniaturisation and advancements in battery life have greatly aided researchers in overcoming challenges and constraints for following free-roaming animals [1, 2]. Avian studies, in particular, have benefited from such advances as researchers can now track migratory routes and gain understanding about wintering areas and connectivity in birds [3,4,5,6]. The predictive models generated by geographical datasets are of considerable importance not only for conservation biologists, but also for other disciplines, for example, veterinary medicine [2]. Despite these benefits, recent research has emphasised the need to identify any possible effects of GPS-tagging before making inferences about the biology of an animal [7,8,9]. Meta-analyses of the impacts of GPS-tags suggest that attaching transmitters and similar devices adversely affects behaviour in a range of bird species [1]. The most substantial effects include increased maintenance behaviour (e.g. preening, fluffing and stretching), restlessness, and energy expenditure as well as decreased likelihood of nesting [1, 10, 11]. How potential adverse effects scale with transmitter weight is still not well known ([1] and references therein). It has been suggested that the effects of transmitters weighing less than 5% of the body weight of an animal would only have negligible effects [12]. However, other studies have suggested that it is advisable not to exceed 3% [13]—but see Tomotani et al. [14] who conclude that using relative logger weight can be a dangerous assumption in general. McMahon et al. [15] assessed four main categories for describing the potential effects of GPS-tags: (1) those originating from capturing an animal, (2) the type of device, including shape, size and colouration, (3) the method applied for mounting the device, and (4) timing and duration of bio-logging.

The evaluation of the impacts of GPS-tags has animal welfare implications, as some animals may experience a stress response to the device [16]. Physiological parameters such as circulating and/or excreted glucocorticoid levels are usually considered a good indicator of the stress response [17,18,19]. Glucocorticoid levels increase in response to stressful situations. This is an endocrine mechanism that aids adaptive defensive response [16], but severe chronic stress may have detrimental effects (e.g. reduced reproductive success or impaired memory; [20, 21]). Thus, additionally to behaviour, physiological measurements might be important in assessing the impact of GPS-loggers on target species. Consideration of such parameters can inform whether the deployment of GPS transmitters is a source of stress for the animal [17,18,19]. Because invasive physiological surveys may themselves be stressful [22,23,24,25,26,27], measuring excreted immune-reactive corticosterone metabolites (CM) provides a non-invasive alternative [28,29,30]. As steroids are metabolised in the liver and excreted into the gut, glucocorticoid metabolite concentrations can be detected in the faeces of mammals or the droppings of birds. Such concentrations have been shown to be representative for the circulating levels [31,32,33,34]. Suedkamp Wells et al. [28] showed that captive Dickcissels (Spiza americana) equipped with bio-loggers in the post-breeding season exhibited elevated CM concentrations in the first 24 h after tagging. Yet excreted glucocorticoid levels returned to baseline levels within 48 h after tagging, which was interpreted as indicating no long-term effects of the attached loggers. GPS-equipped black-legged kittiwakes (Rissa tridactyla) also showed increased plasma corticosterone levels compared to controls during the early chick rearing phase [35]. These findings underscore the need to investigate potential effects of GPS-devices on the physiology and behaviour of study animals.

The aim of this study is to examine the influence of bio-logging on the behaviour and physiological parameters of the Northern Bald Ibis (Geronticus eremita). After being listed as critically endangered for many years, this species is now listed as endangered since 2018. Understanding the effects of GPS-transmitters in this system is potentially important for conservation and reintroduction projects (e.g. in Europe and North Africa) that apply telemetry devices for monitoring and research.

We considered the effect of GPS-loggers in two different contexts: (1) a study on Northern Bald Ibis in captivity focussing on short-term effects of GPS-loggers on behaviour (i.e. maintenance behaviour, locomotion, foraging) and (2) a study on free-ranging Northern Bald Ibis focussing on short- and intermediate-term effects of GPS-loggers on behaviour (i.e. maintenance behaviour, dorsal feather preening, locomotion, foraging) and physiological parameters (i.e. excreted immune-reactive corticosterone metabolites). In both contexts, we expected the strongest effects of the GPS-loggers on behaviour shortly after the tagging procedure, as reported from other studies [36,37,38]. We predicted an increase in maintenance behaviour in GPS-tagged individuals compared with handled birds (experienced handling but not the tagging procedure) or control birds (experienced neither handling nor tagging) since the presence of the device can change the position of the feathers and therefore lead to more self-directed behaviour [39]. We also expected effects of the GPS-transmitters on locomotion and foraging during the first days after attachment because of the increased energy demand caused by the additional weight of the device [39]. In the field, we further tested the effect of the GPS-transmitter on social behaviour (i.e. affiliative and agonistic). In case of a logger effect, we expected GPS-tagged birds to be less engaged in social interactions or even be more frequently a target of agonistic interactions. In addition, excreted immune-reactive corticosterone metabolites were assumed to be elevated in the GPS-tagged and handled birds shortly after attachment of the loggers compared to the control birds. For intermediate-term effects (11 weeks), we predicted that behavioural and physiological values would return to baseline [28, 40]. Furthermore, we investigated whether the GPS-tag has an effect on the body weight of the focal individuals. We expected GPS-tagged birds to show a reduction in body weight as compared to the handled and control groups, due to the increased energy demand of the additional weight and drag of the transmitter [39].

Materials and methods

The aviary context is hereafter presented as “context 1” and the field context as “context 2”.

Field site and study animals

In coordination with the European Breeding Programme (EEP, [41]), a free-ranging Northern Bald Ibis colony was established in 1997 at the Konrad Lorenz Research Centre (KLF, Grünau im Almtal, Austria; 47° 48′ E, 13° 56′ N) by hand-raising zoo-bred chicks [42, 43]. This was the first free-flying Northern Bald Ibis colony northward the Alps after the species became extinct in the seventeenth century. The aim was to implement basic research to gain know-how for reintroduction and conservation purposes [e.g. 2022, 44, 45]. The year-round free-flying birds are housed in a large aviary approximately 20 × 15 × 7 m (L × B × H) at the Cumberland Wildpark where they are able to flutter around and perform short flights. The birds roam the feeding grounds in the Almtal-region, in a radius of 15 km of the aviary, returning for roosting at night and for breeding. Supplementary food (hash made from 1-day-old chicks and beef heart, mixed with insects and soaked dog food) is provided twice a day (0800 and 1500 CET) during winter and early spring when natural resources are limited. The birds are well habituated to the close presence of humans, and each of them is marked with an individual combination of coloured leg rings.

Context 1—aviary

In summer 2013, the aviary was locked for this study for 10 days. At the time of data collection, the colony consisted of 70 individuals, including adult and juvenile birds. Focal animals were 20 birds, chosen randomly with respect to sex (11 females, 9 males) and age (10 adults, i.e. from the 4th year of age; 10 juveniles, i.e. the 1st year after hatching; according to the age classification proposed by Böhm and Pegoraro [46]). Age ranged from 0.5 to 11 years (mean age ± SD = 3.9 ± 3.8). Ten individuals were fitted with GPS transmitters (logger group: Nfemales = 5, Nmales = 5; mean age ± SD = 3.5 ± 3.9) while 10 served as control (handling group: Nfemales = 6, Nmales = 4; mean age ± SD = 3.9 ± 3.9). The list of the focal individuals and their measurements are provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Context 1—aviary

Context 2—field

At the time of data collection, in fall 2017, the colony consisted of 45 individuals, including adult and juvenile birds. Focal animals were 24 adult birds (10 females, 14 males). Their age ranged from 2 to 18 years (mean age ± SD = 7.1 ± 4.5). The focal individuals were assigned to two experimental groups and a control group (8 individuals per group): (i) Logger group (Nfemales = 4, Nmales = 4; mean age ± SD = 7.8 ± 5.1), birds were equipped with GPS transmitters and experienced handling procedure; (ii) Handling group (Nfemales = 3, Nmales = 5; mean age ± SD = 6.6 ± 4.6), birds only experienced handling procedure; (iii) Control group (Nfemales = 3, Nmales = 5; mean age ± SD = 6.9 ± 4.5), birds were neither equipped with GPS transmitters nor experienced handling procedure. The group assignment was done randomly with respect to sex and age. During the period of data collection, the colony was supplemented with food twice in the morning (0745 and 0945 CET; the total amount of food fed in the morning was identical to the one in the afternoon but split into two feeding situations) and once in the afternoon (1500 CET) to facilitate behavioural observations, which started straight after the first morning feeding, and to prevent the individuals from flying away. The list of the focal individuals and their measurements are provided in Table 2.

Table 2 Context 2—field

Data collection

Context 1—aviary

Data collection was performed from 2 to 12 July 2013 (10 days) and was divided into three phases (Table 3): phase 1, a 4-day-long phase to collect baseline observations from the behaviour of the birds in the aviary; phase 2, a 2-day-long treatment phase (i.e. capturing and equipping); phase 3, a further 4-day-long post-treatment observation phase.

Table 3 Phases of the data collection

Birds were caught on two consecutive days (phase 2) between 0930 and 1430 CET. Captures were done by hand or by using a hand net and avoiding chasing. Several morphological measurements (including body weight for the present study) were taken for different research purposes from all focal individuals. Weighing of individuals ensured that the transmitter did not exceed 3% of the body weight of the bird ([13]; Table 1), ranging between 1.6 and 2.4% of the body weight of the single individuals. Ten birds were fitted with a GPS-transmitter. The entire procedure (from catching to releasing) lasted between 15 and 25 min per individual (mean handling durations ± SD: handling group = 20.7 ± 3.5, logger group = 20.6 ± 2.4).

Context 2—field

Behavioural data and individual droppings for excreted CM were collected from 25 October 2017 to 11 January 2018 (79 days). Data collection was divided into five phases (Table 3): phase 1, an 11-day-long pre-treatment phase to collect baseline behavioural observations and physiological measurements; phase 2, a 1-day-long treatment phase (handling procedure and transmitter attachment); phase 3, a 10-day-long post-treatment data collection phase; phase 4, a 5-day-long post-treatment phase to perform data collection 1 month later; phase 5, a 5-day-long post-treatment phase to perform data collection 2 months later. During phase 2, only droppings for CM analysis were collected; behavioural observations did not take place, as the GPS-transmitters were attached in the morning.

Birds (logger and handling groups) were caught on 1 day (phase 2) between 0815 and 1100 CET. Captures were done by hand or by using a hand net and avoiding chasing. Body weight was taken as a morphological measurement on the day of transmitter attachment (phase 2) and at the end of the experiment (phase 5). Weighing of individuals ensured that the transmitter did not exceed 3% of the body weight of the birds ([13]; Table 2), ranging between 1.49 and 1.88% of the body weight of the single individuals [13]. For further statistical analysis, we calculated the weight change (Δbody weight) between phase 2 and phase 5. Eight ibises were fitted with a GPS-transmitter. The entire procedure (from catching to releasing) lasted between 13 and 30 min per individual (mean handling durations ± SD: handling group = 18.6 ± 4.7, logger group = 19.6 ± 4.0).

Transmitter attachment

Focal animals of the logger group of both contexts (i.e. in captivity and in the field) were fitted with telemetry devices (Ecotone® Telemetry, Sopot, Poland; http://ecotone-telemetry.com/en), which were backpack-mounted with a harness following an earlier study by Lindsell et al. ([47], see also Tables 1 and 2). All birds were equipped with either GSM-GPS transmitters, which store the GPS locations and transmit them via GSM network to a server, or UHF-GPS transmitters, from which stored GPS-data can be downloaded via UHF-antenna. All loggers were equipped with solar panels to recharge the batteries.

Context 1—aviary

Four birds were equipped with GSM-GPS transmitters (Ecotone transmitter 1: weight 28 g, approximately 2.3% of the body weight of the birds ranging between 1170 and 1270 g; Table 1). Six ibises were outfitted with UHF-GPS transmitters (Ecotone transmitter 2: weight 22 g, approximately 1.8% of the body weight of the birds ranging between 1100 and 1340 g; Table 1).

Context 2—field

All focal birds in the logger group were equipped with GSM-GPS transmitters (Ecotone transmitter 1: weight 20 g, approximately 1.5% of the body weight of the birds ranging between 1300 and 1340 g; Ecotone transmitter 2: weight 22 g, approximately 1.8% of the body weight of the birds ranging between 1170 and 1260 g; Table 2).

In both contexts, the loggers were not removed from the focal individuals after data collection for approximately further 8 months; however, logistical and organisational issues did not allow to further investigate the effects of the deployment.

Behavioural data

Context 1—aviary

During the 8 days of phase 1 and 3, every focal individual was video-recorded (Canon Legria FS306) for 10 min twice per day, in the morning between 0900 and 1300 CET and in the afternoon between 1300 and 1800 CET, considering a break of at least 2 h between repeated observations of the same individual. In total, 16 protocols were collected per individual, adding up to a sum of 3000 min of observation. Due to technical problems some videos of phase 3 got partly lost (on average 6.9 min per individual). This was taken into account in the analysis. Videos were analysed using the software Solomon Coder beta (©2013 András Péter). The following behavioural parameters were coded and analysed: duration of locomotion (including walking and flying), frequency of foraging (including drinking, feeding and poking with the bill in the soil), and frequency of maintenance behaviour (including preening, scratching, shaking, stretching, bathing in the sun or in the water; for an exhaustive description of the ethogram of the Northern Bald Ibis see [48]). Videos were coded by JG and ML after calculation of inter-observer reliability using Kappa statistics (Kappa = 8.3, “almost perfect agreement”; [49]).

Context 2—field

During phases 1, 3, 4 and 5 behavioural observations of focal individuals were collected with the software Prim8 Mobile (mobile computing to record nature, http://www.prim8software.com/; [50]) by applying focal sampling with a continuous recording method [51, 52]. All observations were taken once per day between 0830 and 1200 CET, with each protocol lasting 5 min per individual. The simultaneous observation of pair partners was avoided to prevent pseudo-replication in social interactions. In total, 719 protocols were collected (on average: \(\bar{x}\) ± SE = 30 ± 2 focal observations per individual). The following behavioural parameters were coded and analysed: duration of maintenance behaviour (including preening, scratching, shaking, fluffing, stretching, sleeping, resting, bathing in the sun or water), dorsal feather preening (preening the area around the logger), locomotion (walking, short distance flights with the focal individual still in sight) and foraging (drinking, feeding, poking with the bill in the soil), as well as frequency of initiated and received affiliative (i.e. greeting, preening, preening invitation, mutual bill shaking, contact sitting) and agonistic (i.e. displacing, threatening, pecking, fighting) interactions (for an exhaustive description of the ethogram of the Northern Bald Ibis see [48]). Behavioural data were collected by VP-S and TC after calculation of inter-observer reliability using intraclass correlation coefficient (package “irr”, [53]; coefficient = 0.953, “excellent reliability”, [54]). A ratio per minute was calculated for the durations and the frequencies.

Collection of droppings and analysis of corticosterone metabolites

Context 2—field

To determine concentrations of excreted CM, individual droppings were collected. Droppings represent an integrated, proportional record of the plasma corticosterone levels depending on the gut passage time [32], which we know to be 2–3 h [55], similar to the records on white ibises (Eudocimus albus, [56]). Daily sample collection was conducted independently of behavioural observations. To account for possible endogenous diurnal variations, droppings were collected from 1600 to 2000 (CET) each day. The collected sample was transferred into an individual Eppendorf® microtube (Eppendorf®, Hamburg, Germany) directly after defecation of the focal bird to avoid cross-contamination with other droppings. The samples were stored on ice during collection and within 3 h frozen at − 20 °C for CM analysis. In total, we collected 591 droppings for CM determination (on average: \(\bar{x}\) ± SE = 25 ± 3 droppings per individual).

The analysis was done via an enzyme immunoassay (EIA; [31, 32, 57]) suitable for Northern Bald Ibises [55] at the laboratory of the Department of Behavioural Biology, University of Vienna (Austria). The intra- and interassay coefficients of variance amounted to 9.57% and 5.54%, respectively.

The measured value of nanogram CM concentration per gramme dropping was taken into further statistical analysis.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were carried out using the software R 3.4.0 [58] and the packages “lme4” [59], “glmmADMB” [60] and “MuMIn” [61]. We checked whether the residuals were normally distributed through visual inspection and a Shapiro–Wilk test. We used an information-theoretic approach and calculated all possible candidate models, ranked them according to their AICc values (second-order form of Akaike’s Information Criterion to account for small sample sizes; [62]) and selected the models with ∆AICc ≤ 2 with respect to the top-ranked model for model averaging in order to create model-averaged coefficients [63].

Context 1—aviary

We defined (1) locomotion, (2) foraging and (3) maintenance behaviour as dependent variables. Generalised linear mixed models (GLMM) were used to investigate whether phase 2, i.e. the 2 days of catching and fitting the birds with GPS-transmitters, had an effect on the behavioural categories. In each set of candidate models the frequency or proportion of one behavioural category served as dependent variable with the following fixed factors in each full model: phase (pre- or post-catching), relative weight of the GPS-transmitter (i.e. percentage of the body weight of the individual; for the handled birds this was zero), sex and age class (adult, juvenile), time of day (i.e. morning and afternoon). We included the phase (i.e. 1 or 3) and the relative weight of the transmitter as interaction. Regarding locomotion, we fitted a beta distribution (link = logit); i.e. locomotion was measured as the proportion of observation time. The dependent variable “foraging” contained 133 zeros, and the remaining 170 values varied widely; therefore, it was converted into a binary variable, i.e. foraging or not foraging (family = binomial, link = logit). A negative binomial distribution (link = log) was fitted on the dependent variable “maintenance behaviour” (frequency). The identity of the individual and the day of observation (1 to 8) were added as random factors to all models.

Context 2—field

The following parameters were defined as response variables: (1) behaviour (including maintenance behaviour, dorsal feather preening, locomotion, foraging, social interactions), (2) CM and (3) Δbody weight. Behavioural categories were treated as separate dependent variables in the candidate models. Fixed factors in each full model were experimental group, phase and the interaction term between those two parameters. Sex was not included as a fixed factor, as no effect was found in context 1 regarding logger attachment. Subject identities were included as random factors in all models to control for between subject variation and unbalanced design. GLMMs with an inverse gaussian distribution (link = log) were used to assess the effect of GPS transmitter attachment on behaviour. As the inverse gaussian distribution is only able to run with positive values (> 0), we added the number 1 to each behavioural category. To investigate the effect on CM and Δbody weight, we used Linear mixed-effects models.

Results

Context 1—aviary

Locomotion

Age class was the most important predictor, i.e. juveniles moved more than adults (Additional file 1: Tables S1 and S2). Compared to age class, the relative importance of pre/post-catching (i.e. phase 1 and 3), time of day and sex was very low; thus, these factors had a less important effect on locomotion (Additional file 1: Table S2). The interaction term was not included in the top-ranked models (Additional file 1: Table S1). Most importantly, the relative weight of the GPS-transmitters did not occur in the best models; accordingly, the presence and weight of a transmitter were not found to affect the behaviour.

Foraging

Age class and time of day were the most important predictors, i.e. juveniles were more likely to forage than adults; foraging was more likely to be observed in the afternoon than in the morning (Additional file 1: Table S2). Compared to age class and time of day, all other parameters (i.e. sex, pre/post-catching and relative transmitter weight) had a much lower relative importance and therefore there is little evidence that these factors influenced foraging (Additional file 1: Table S2). The interaction term was not included in the top-ranked models (Additional file 1: Table S1).

Maintenance behaviour

Time of day was the most important predictor, i.e. in the afternoon maintenance behaviour was observed more frequently (Additional file 1: Table S2). The factor sex also had relatively high importance with 0.82, i.e. females showed less maintenance behaviour than males. The other parameters pre/post-catching, relative transmitter weight and age class all had very low relative importance, meaning that there is little evidence in this dataset that these fixed factors influenced the frequency of maintenance behaviour (Additional file 1: Table S2). The interaction term was not included in the top-ranked models (Additional file 1: Table S1).

Context 2—field

Locomotion, foraging and maintenance behaviour

Phase (i.e. the different phases of the data collection) was the most influential variable regarding the response variables maintenance behaviour, dorsal feather preening, locomotion, foraging (Additional file 2: Tables S3 and S4). Locomotion and foraging (Fig. 1) declined throughout phases 1 to 4 and showed an increase in phase 5. The duration of dorsal feather preening only slightly changed during the experiment, whereas a peak in maintenance behaviour was observed in phase 4 (5-day-long post-treatment phase 1 month later) with decreasing durations in phase 5 (5-day-long post-treatment phase 2 months later; Fig. 1). Experimental group and the interaction term were not included in the top-ranked model.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Context 2—field. Mean estimated durations (s) per minute of locomotion, foraging, maintenance behaviour as well as dorsal feather preening. For a detailed description of the phases, see Table 3. Solid line, maintenance behaviour; dotted line, dorsal feather preening; shortdash, locomotion; longdash, foraging. N = 24

Social behaviour

Model-averaged results identified phase, experimental group and the interaction term between the two parameters as the strongest determinants of initiated and received affiliative behaviour (Additional file 2: Tables S3 and S4). The affiliative behaviours (initiated and received) increased initially in the GPS-tagged birds compared with the handled and control birds; then we observed a decline in the GPS-tagged birds during phase 4. Furthermore, birds in the control (initiated affiliative behaviour) and handled (initiated and received affiliative behaviour) groups showed a peak during phase 3 and 4, respectively; in both cases, the frequencies decreased afterwards (Figs. 2 and 3). Received agonistic behaviour was best explained by phase, with decreasing frequencies throughout phases 1 to 4 and increasing ones during phase 5 (Additional file 2: Tables S3 and S4). Experimental group and the interaction term were not included in the top-ranked model. Candidate models with initiated agonistic behaviour as response variable did not improve penalised model fit over the null model, as assessed by AICc, indicating that variation in the data cannot be explained by any of the fixed factors.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Context 2—field. Mean estimated frequency of initiated affiliative behaviour in relation to the different phases and experimental groups. For a detailed description of the phases, see Table 3. NLoggerGroup = 8; NHandlingGroup = 8; NControlGroup = 8

Fig. 3
figure 3

Context 2—field. Mean estimated frequency of received affiliative behaviour in relation to the different phases and experimental groups. For a detailed description of the phases, see Table 3. NLoggerGroup = 8; NHandlingGroup = 8; NControlGroup = 8

Corticosterone metabolites

The excretion pattern of CM was best explained by phase, experimental group and the interaction term of these fixed factors (Additional file 2: Tables S3 and S4). CM levels of the GPS-tagged birds increased steadily after GPS-transmitter attachment, whereas a decline was observed within the handled birds (Fig. 4). On the contrary, the control birds showed first an increase in CM levels during phase 3, with decreasing levels afterwards. However, during phase 5, all three experimental groups showed similar CM concentrations.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Context 2—field. Mean estimated CM (ng/g) in relation to the different phases and experimental groups. For a detailed description of the phases, see Table 3. NLoggerGroup = 8; NHandlingGroup = 8; NControlGroup = 8

ΔBody weight

The full model did not improve penalised model fit over the null model, as assessed by AICc, indicating that variation in the data cannot be explained by any of these factors.

Discussion

The deployment of GPS-transmitters on Northern Bald Ibises did not cause remarkable changes in measured behaviour. However, excreted corticosterone metabolites (CM) increased after transmitter attachment during month 2 before returning to baseline levels during month 3. Our results indicate that the GPS-transmitters used in the present study (i.e. up to approximately 2.5% of the body weight of an animal) did not affect foraging, locomotion, maintenance behaviour and dorsal feather preening or received agonistic behaviour in captive and free-flying Northern Bald Ibises during the immediate post-tagging period. The pre- and post-catching phase as well as the relative GPS-transmitter weight in aviary birds (context 1) and the variable “experimental group” in the field (context 2) had no or only low relative importance as compared to other factors such as age class, time of day (context 1) or phase (context 2). These outcomes contradict our expectations, as we expected to find the strongest differences between tagged birds and handled and control birds shortly after catching and tagging. In regard to the aviary study (context 1), perhaps behavioural acclimatisation after logger attachment was facilitated compared to free-flying conditions, as not much energy had to be expended for foraging activities. Both weather conditions and GPS transmitters have been shown to affect the energy costs of behaviour [39, 64]. Such constraints can be reflected in elevated cost of foraging [65] or in behavioural response that minimises such costs [66]. Because locomotion, foraging and maintenance behaviour in free-flying Northern Bald Ibis did not seem to be affected by logger deployment in general, we may conclude that the harness-attached GPS-transmitters in the present study did not have a negative impact on the behaviour observed. Furthermore, we can likely exclude a possible effect of handling time, as the mean values of procedure duration were similar between the experimental groups.

Despite the overall finding of little measurable effect of the GPS-transmitter on behaviour, affiliative behaviour decreased in the GPS-tagged group after the attachment as compared to the handled and control groups, indicating that the tagged birds experienced some impact after logger deployment. Even though we did not investigate the social network [67] of these birds, one possible explanation for the observed pattern is that GPS-tagged birds moved towards the edge of the network for a short period, and for this reason they initiated and received less affiliative behaviour compared with birds in the handled and control groups. Such behavioural responses could negatively impact reproductive behaviour as breeders are usually better embedded in the social network as compared to non-breeders [68], but this remains to be tested. One of the most substantial effects of GPS-tagging reported in other studies is the decreased likelihood of nesting [1]. A meta-analysis found the strongest negative effects on reproduction in individuals tagged with neck collars [69]. As we found a clear difference between the handled group and the GPS-tagged group, we can conclude that the observed effect was caused by the logger deployment alone and not due to the handling experience. We can further exclude CM levels as a reason for the decline in affiliative behaviour, as the GPS-tagged group excreted similar concentrations as the handled and control birds during phase 3. Even though the behavioural effect was not long-lasting, a careful consideration of the type and period (i.e. reproductive vs non-reproductive period) of GPS-logger attachment is pertinent.

Our results hint at an effect of GPS-loggers on CM excretion. During phases 1 to 3, all experimental groups showed similar patterns in CM levels. The low value in the control group during phase 2 could be related to the small sample size, resulting in the differences in CM concentrations between the groups. As a seasonal effect, CM levels generally decrease in late fall and increase again towards the onset of the mating season [70]; however, this was not the case during phase 4 (December) in the GPS-tagged birds as compared to the handled and control birds. Notably, the CM concentrations in all three experimental groups were comparable at the end of the experiment (January), and therefore this might be considered an intermediate-term effect of GPS-tagging. A seasonal or handling effect on CM level can be excluded as the GPS-tagged birds showed an increase in CM concentrations, whereas the handled birds showed similar values to the control group. Further, we can exclude an impact of the GPS-logger deployment on CM due to sex or age, as those parameters were taken into account when designing the experimental setup. The impact of the GPS-loggers on CM could be a consequence of our small sample size. However, we accounted for this possibility when defining and choosing the statistical models. Thus, we tend to exclude this possibility, even though confidence in the pattern would benefit from a greater sample size generating more robust results. Glucocorticoid concentrations have been shown to increase with handling time [71]. Therefore, we cannot exclude a possible effect of individual differences in reacting to stressful situations; for instance, small differences in handling time between individuals might have affected the results. Furthermore, we also cannot exclude potential impacts on the flight performance of the GPS-tagged birds as we did not measure flight behaviour. A recent study showed that flight speed reduces depending on how heavy the bird is after tagging [14], which would certainly have an impact on wild birds. Thus, a more thorough consideration of flight performance would be necessary. Still, the detected effect in the GPS-tagged birds could have consequences for their subsequent reproductive success, due to the possible increase in energy expenditure during the winter period. Under natural conditions such increased energetic expenditure during winter could eventuate in less available energy allocated to reproduction, i.e. building a nest, producing eggs and raising chicks. We did not detect changes in body weight in relation to logger deployment in the present study, and therefore one could argue that GPS-tagged individuals in this study had enough energy available for survival and investment into the breeding season.

Compared to the adult birds, juveniles showed more locomotion and foraging behaviour. Both foraging and maintenance behaviours occurred more often in the afternoon than in the morning, which was independent of the presence of a GPS-transmitter. Thus, when testing the effects of GPS-loggers in animals, it is important to account for different age classes and time of day that could mask variation caused by transmitter effects. A detailed discussion of these results is beyond the scope of this study. However, there is evidence from other studies that juvenile and sub-adult birds had lower foraging efficiency compared with adults and experienced individuals, which may force juveniles to migrate later than adults [47, 72]. In our study, yearling juvenile birds were observed foraging more frequently than adult birds, irrespective of being equipped with a transmitter or not. The hierarchy within the colony is another factor worth considering: sub-adults and especially juveniles after fledging (as in our case) are low in rank and often get displaced by adult birds, who pose a risk to scrounge their food [48, 73]. Thus, age and time of day can have significant effects that require scrutiny per study species when tagging is considered.

Conclusions

This study was performed on a globally endangered species, for which the kind of data that can be collected by GPS telemetry could be essential to manage its conservation. For example, GPS technology is used to monitor endangered species, their threats and to protect their habitats [74] or to detect poaching events [75]. At the same time, given the small population size, individual birds of this species are disproportionally important for reproduction. For both reasons, it is highly relevant to identify and minimise potential effects of GPS-loggers on this species. In the present study, we found no long-term effects of GPS-transmitters below 3% of the body weight of an animal on locomotion, foraging, maintenance and agonistic behaviour in the Northern Bald Ibis. However, affiliative behaviour and the excretion pattern of CM were temporarily affected by tagging. Our results imply that a closer look at physiological parameters is important to detect whether there is an effect on the stress level of the GPS-tagged animals, even though no behavioural changes might be observed after logger deployment. These findings are relevant for conservation and management projects running on species that include the use of animal-carried bio-loggers.