Background

Multiple physical and mental health benefits can be attained when children participate in the recommended 60 min per day of moderate- to vigorous-intensity physical activity [1, 2]. Despite these benefits, population based-studies have reported that over 50% of children in Australia and internationally are not meeting recommendations [3,4,5,6]. Schools are considered ideal settings for the promotion of children’s physical activity. There are multiple opportunities for children to be physically active over the course of the school week, including during break times, sport, Physical Education class and active travel to and from school. Studies have shown interventions targeting these discrete periods may be effective in increasing children’s physical activity levels [7, 8], with the potential to contribute to up to 50% of the physical activity required to meet physical activity guidelines [9]. However, with limited time available during these discrete periods, additional opportunities may be required in order for children to achieve the recommended levels of physical activity. Classroom-based physical activity provides another way for children to be active at school. This involves classroom teachers incorporating physical activity into class time through either integrating physical activity into lessons (physically active lessons), or adding short bursts of physical activity, either with curriculum content (curriculum focused active breaks) or without (active breaks).

There is increasing interest from researchers and education professionals about the potential for classroom-based physical activity to positively impact academic-related outcomes, including classroom behaviour, cognitive function and academic achievement. While some teachers express concern that classroom-based physical activity may have an adverse effect on on-task classroom behaviour [10], emerging evidence from systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest that overall physical activity may have a small positive effect on on-task classroom behaviour [11,12,13,14,15,16,17]. There is less evidence on classroom-based physical activity.

Narrative reviews [18,19,20], one systematic review [21] and two meta-analyses [22, 23] have explored the impact of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic-related outcomes. However, these were narrow in scope, included few studies, and combined findings among primary and secondary school students, which may be problematic due to the difference in education settings.

A systematic review of 11 studies concluded that physically active lessons may have a positive effect, or no effect on academic-related outcomes [21]. However, that study did not consider other forms of classroom-based physical activity (e.g. active breaks), combined findings among primary and secondary school students, and did not include a meta-analysis [21].

A meta-analysis of four intervention studies found that classroom-based physical activity had a positive effect on academic-related outcomes (M = 0.67; 95%CI:0.26,1.09) [23]. Similar results were reported in a meta-analysis of 24 intervention studies investigating the association between different types of physical activity (e.g., during recess or lunch vs. active breaks vs. physically active lessons) and school engagement (behaviour at home and at school, and emotions, e.g. lesson enjoyment) [22]. In that meta-analysis, overall results showed physical activity had a significant positive effect on school engagement (d = 0.28;95%CI:0.12,0.46) [22]. When broken down into type of physical activity, active breaks (n = 4 studies) appeared to be the most effective type of intervention for improving school engagement (d = 0.55; 95%CI:0.02,1.06), compared with recess or lunch time physical activity (n = 3 studies; d = 0.26; 95%CI:-0.19,0.73) and physically active lessons (n = 5 studies; d = 0.22; 95%CI: -0.21,0.66) [22]. However, results from those meta-analyses are limited by the small number of included studies [22, 23], the narrow range of potential academic-related outcomes assessed, the combination of findings among primary and secondary school students [22], and their recency [23].

The current paper aims to expand on findings from these reviews by conducting a systematic review and meta-analyses of the evidence of effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions (active breaks, curriculum-focused active breaks and physically active lessons) on a broad range of academic-related outcomes (classroom behavior, cognitive function and academic achievement), specifically among primary school-aged children. A secondary aim is to examine the effect of these interventions on children’s physical activity levels.

Methods

Definitions

While there are no set definitions for classroom-based physical activity, the following definitions are provided in order to maintain consistency and clarity throughout the remainder of this systematic review.

Classroom-based physical activity: physical activity carried out during regular class time, and can occur either inside or outside the classroom (e.g. hallway, playground), and is distinct from school recess/lunch break times. Classroom-based physical activity can take three forms:

  • Active breaks: short bouts of physical activity performed as a break from academic instruction [24].

  • Curriculum-focussed active breaks: short bouts of physical activity that include curriculum content [25, 26].

  • Physically active lessons: the integration of physical activity into lessons in key learning areas other than physical education (e.g. mathematics) [27, 28].

Academic-related outcomes: overarching term to encompass factors associated with academic performance at school. These can be grouped into three main categories:

  • Classroom behaviour: Observed behaviours that may promote or interfere with learning in the classroom, including on-task behaviour [29] (e.g. concentrating on tasks assigned by the teacher), and off-task behaviour (e.g. not concentrating on tasks assigned by the teacher).

  • Cognitive function: Mental process (e.g. executive function) that may influence academic performance [29].

  • Academic achievement: A child’s performance on school-related tasks; often reported via classroom grades, national standardised tests or progress monitoring tools [29], as well as self-reported perceived academic competence [30].

Registration and protocol

This study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) recommendations for systematic review reporting, and was registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) (record #CRD42016027294).

Search strategy

Studies were identified through a systematic search of four electronic databases (PubMed, ERIC, SPORTDiscus and PsycINFO), first conducted in January 2016, and updated in January 2017 by one author (AW). The search strategy consisted of four elements (see Table 1). The search was limited to peer-reviewed articles published in English in all available years. ‘Grey’ literature, including the reference lists from the websites of two organisations (“Active Academics” and “Active Living Research”) involved in children’s physical activity research were also searched.

Table 1 Article search terms and databases searched

Inclusion criteria

A predetermined set of inclusion criteria were used to select papers for this systematic review. Each study had to meet the following criteria:

  1. 1.

    Intervention study design;

  2. 2.

    Investigated associations between classroom-based physical activity and at least one academic-related outcome. Interventions involving strategies in addition to classroom-based physical activity were excluded (to enable the effects of classroom-based physical activity to be isolated);

  3. 3.

    Study population included primary school-aged children (5–12 years);

  4. 4.

    Presented original data;

  5. 5.

    Did not focus specifically on special populations (e.g. overweight children).

Study selection

The search yielded 7729 citations from electronic database records, and 17 from ‘grey’ literature (Fig. 1). After removing duplicates (n = 500), the titles and/or abstracts of 7246 unique publications were screened by one author (AW). A total of 101 publications were identified as potentially relevant according to the inclusion criteria. Full texts of 98 of these 101 articles were obtained and reviewed independently by two authors to determine eligibility (AW, KB). Two full texts were conference abstracts only, and one full-text was unable to be retrieved despite extensive librarian-assisted enquiries and emails directly to the contact author. Of the 98 full-text articles, a total of 59 were excluded as not meeting inclusion criteria. Disagreements between the two reviewers were resolved through discussion with all authors. Reference lists of included articles were also examined, however no additional studies were identified. Thirty-nine unique citations satisfied the eligibility criteria and were included in this systematic review.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA Flow Diagram showing flow of studies through the review process

Data extraction

Paper characteristics including country of study, study design, participant characteristics, intervention characteristics, academic-related outcome measures, physical activity measures, and results were extracted by one author (AW). Interventions were then categorised as active break, curriculum focussed active break, or physically active lesson intervention.

Methodological quality

Two authors (AW, KB) independently assessed the methodological quality of the included studies using the Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) tool [31]. This six-component rating scale for interventions assesses (1) selection bias; (2) study design; (3) confounders; (4) blinding; (5) data collection methods; and (6) withdrawals and drop outs. Each component was rated on a three-point scale as either strong, moderate or weak using the tool’s defined criteria. Based on these ratings, an overall methodological quality score was given; either strong (no weak component ratings); moderate (one weak component rating); or weak (more than one weak component rating), following the tool’s accompanying instructions. Where disagreements existed, deliberation occurred until a consensus was reached.

Meta-analyses

Meta-analyses were conducted where there were at least three studies investigating the same broad outcome, i.e. classroom behaviour, cognitive function, or academic achievement. Due to heterogeneity across study designs, for inclusion studies were required to have a separate comparison group (i.e. RCT or quasi experimental with control group). Studies that used a within subject or cross over study design were therefore excluded from meta-analysis.

To avoid duplication of studies under a single outcome, where studies reported intervention effects on multiple measures for an outcome (this happened only for cognitive functions) [32, 33] a decision was made to include outcomes relating to executive functions, over memory. Executive functions, inhibition in particular, have been shown to be consistently related to academic achievement [34] and therefore were considered salient to teachers. Thus, where inhibition and memory were reported, only inhibition was included in the meta-analysis; where executive functions and short term memory were reported, only executive functions were included in the meta-analysis. Typically higher scores were reflective of better academic-related outcomes. Where lower scores reflected better academic-related outcomes these scores were reversed.

As academic achievement tools varied widely in quality, only studies using national standardised tests or progress monitoring tools were included in the meta-analyses. Further, intervention effects on mathematics were used when studies reported multiple subject assessments, as math was the most commonly reported outcome. Of the 39 studies included in this systematic review, 16 were included in meta-analyses. Reasons for exclusion were: insufficient data for calculating effect sizes and authors did not respond to email requests for additional data (n = 6), using a within subject or cross-over study design (n = 9), not including a separate comparison group (n = 2), insufficient studies investigating an outcome (n = 4), or only reporting results separately for subgroups (e.g. BMI categories) (n = 2).

Analysis

Meta-analyses were conducted using Review Manager 5.3. The wide variation in interventions and academic-related outcomes employed in the different studies warranted use of a random effects model. Effect sizes (standardised mean difference) were computed as the difference between treatment and control means.

Results

Of the 39 studies identified, 19 examined the effect of active breaks [24, 26, 35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51], seven examined curriculum-focussed active breaks [25, 52,53,54,55,56,57], and thirteen examined physically active lessons [27, 28, 32, 33, 58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66] on academic-related outcomes. The majority of studies (n = 27) were published in or after 2014 [24, 26, 32, 33, 36, 39,40,41, 43, 46,47,48,49,50,51, 57, 65, 66], and none before 2006. Most (n = 18) were conducted in the USA [25, 36, 39, 40, 42, 44, 45, 51,52,53,54,55, 57,58,59,60, 64, 65], seven in the Netherlands [32, 41, 49, 50, 61, 62, 66], four in Australia [27, 28, 46, 47], three in Canada [24, 35, 43], two in Scotland [37, 38], and one each in South Africa [48], UK [63], Greece [56] Denmark [33], and Switzerland [26]. Sample sizes ranged from 14 [60] to over 4500 participants [45], with sample sizes <300 in the majority of studies (n = 28) [24,25,26,27,28, 33, 35, 39,40,41, 43, 44, 46,47,48,49,50,51, 53, 55,56,57, 59,60,61,62,63,64]. Intervention periods spanned from single lessons [49, 55, 59, 65] to 3 year duration [58], with most lasting no longer than nine weeks (n = 23) [24,25,26,27,28, 33, 37,38,39,40,41, 43,44,45,46, 48, 50, 52, 55,56,57, 59, 63]. Study information is presented in Table 2 (active breaks), Table 3 (curriculum focused active breaks) and Table 4 (physically active lessons).

Table 2 Studies investigating the effect of active breaks on physical activity and academic-related outcomes
Table 3 Studies investigating the effect of curriculum focussed active breaks on academic and physical activity outcomes
Table 4 Studies investigating the effect of physically active lessons on academic and physical activity outcomes

Intervention content

There was considerable variation across studies in intervention content. While most (12 out of 19) active break interventions featured basic aerobic movements that students could be performed in their classroom (e.g. jumping jacks), and required no set-up or equipment [24, 35,36,37,38,39,40, 42, 43, 45, 50, 51], others were performed outside the classroom (e.g. sports field) [26, 41, 46,47,48], and/or required additional equipment (e.g. markers, skipping ropes, balls, exercise bands, dance videos, or specialised stacking cups) [41, 44, 46, 49]. One study utilised both cognitively engaging active breaks (i.e. physical activity combined with cognitive demand) and active breaks to explore separate and combined effects of physical activity and cognitive engagement on cognitive function [26]. The target frequency, duration and physical activity intensity of the breaks varied, ranging from 4 min of vigorous-intensity physical activity weekly [24, 43] to 20 min of moderate intensity physical activity done twice per day [49].

There was more consistency in content across curriculum-focussed active breaks, compared with the active breaks without curriculum content. All curriculum-focussed active breaks featured physical activity integrated into a combination of key learning areas, including mathematics, language, science and/or social studies, and aimed to reinforce previously taught lesson content [25, 52,53,54,55,56,57]. Further, most (5 out of 7) required daily participation in 10 to 20 min of physical activity [19, 52,53,54, 57]. When specified, participation was required at a moderate-[56] or moderate-to vigorous-physical activity intensity [55], but intensity was not specified in the majority (5 out of 7) of these studies [25, 52,53,54, 57].

While curriculum-focussed active breaks aimed to reinforce previously taught lesson content, physically active lessons were used to teach new lesson content [27, 28, 32, 33, 58,59,60,61,62, 64,65,66]. These lessons predominately incorporated physical activity into mathematics and/or language lessons, but some also incorporated science and/or social studies [27, 28, 32, 33, 58,59,60,61,62, 64,65,66]. Lessons ranged in duration from 30 to 60 min [27, 28, 32, 33, 60,61,62,63,64, 66] with most (8 out of 13) requiring participation three times per week [27, 28, 32, 33, 61, 62, 64, 66]. Other physically active lessons were described as single lessons as part of pilot interventions [59, 63, 65], or stipulated physical activity time per week, rather than number of lessons per week [58].

Intervention fidelity

Intervention fidelity was reported in twelve studies. For the three active break interventions delivered by teachers, various measures of fidelity were used, however, no study clearly reported compliance with implementing active breaks daily or the number of active break sessions conducted. Active break interventions delivered by research staff reported high fidelity, showing most children achieved the required physical activity intensity [39,40,41], or at least 50% of each intervention session was spent at the required intensity [46, 47].

For physically active lesson interventions, teacher reports showed they delivered lessons either as intended [27] or for at least 50% of the required minutes per week [58]. Similar to active break studies, when delivered by research staff, at least 60% of intervention lessons were spent at the required physical activity intensity [61, 62]. No curriculum focussed active break study reported fidelity.

Methodological quality

Of the 39 identified studies, most (36 out of 39) received a moderate [24, 26, 33, 38,39,40,41, 48, 53, 55,56,57, 60, 62], or weak quality rating score [25, 27, 28, 35,36,37, 42, 44,45,46,47, 49,50,51,52, 54, 59, 61, 63,64,65,66]. Three received a strong quality rating score [32, 43, 58]. Low to moderate quality score ratings were mostly attributable to not reporting or controlling for relevant demographic confounders, not reporting blinding of participants and researchers, and not reporting participant attrition. Further, for many studies, authors did not report the rate of participant or school participation. See Appendix A for further detail on quality assessment of included studies.

Academic-related outcomes: Classroom behaviour

Studies assessed the effect of participation in these programs on academic-related outcomes both immediately following participation in a session (acute) and after a longer exposure (chronic; e.g. pre- and post- intervention periods spanning up to 8 months). Regardless of type of classroom-based physical activity, the majority of studies (10 out of 12) showed participation in these programs had an acute effect on improving on-task classroom behaviour [25, 27, 28, 39, 52, 57, 62, 65] and reducing off-task behaviour [36, 43] However, evidence in the few studies with longer term follow-up (2 out of 2 studies) suggest that this improvement may dissipate over time, with no difference between groups when chronic intervention effects on reported behaviour incidents were assessed [42, 47]. Due to few studies investigating chronic effects of classroom-based physical activity on on-task and off task classroom behaviour (<5) it was not possible to separate acute and chronic effects in the meta-analysis. Results from the 4 included studies show classroom-based physical activity had a positive effect on improving on-task behaviour and reducing off-task behaviour (standardised mean difference = 0.60 (95% CI: 0.20,1.00)) (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on classroom behaviour

Academic-related outcomes: Cognitive function

Studies also assessed acute and chronic effects of classroom-based physical activity on a range of cognitive functions [24, 32, 37, 38, 40, 41, 46, 47, 49, 50, 54, 64]. Results showed active breaks had an acute positive effect on selective attention (3 out of 4 studies) [24, 41, 49]. No acute effect was reported for sustained attention [46], information processing [50] or focussed attention, processing speed and accuracy [26], and no chronic effect was reported for planning, attention, simultaneous or successive cognitive processes [47] or executive function [32]. Acute intervention effects on executive function were inconsistent, with no difference between groups reported in one study [40], while another reported improvements in executive function but only for those receiving the intervention in the second week of delivery [37, 38]. Results were also inconsistent for chronic intervention effects on fluid intelligence, with one study reporting a significant improvement after 3 months [64], while another reported no difference between groups after 1-year [54]. Due to few studies reporting chronic effects of participation (<5) results for acute and chronic studies were combined in the meta-analysis (5 studies). Results from the meta-analysis indicate classroom-based physical activity had no effect on cognitive function (standardised mean difference = 0.33 (95% CI: -0.11,0.77) (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on cognitive function

Academic-related outcomes: Academic achievement

Studies assessed intervention effects on academic achievement using a range of academic assessment tools, including standardised tests, progress monitoring tools, grades and content recall quizzes. Reported effects on academic achievement varied by intervention duration and the type of assessment tool used. Interventions of shorter duration tended to show improvement in academic achievement if a progress monitoring tool was used, but not if a national standardised test was used. Seven out of 8 studies using a progress monitoring tool reported significant improvement in academic achievement following intervention periods ranging from 4 weeks to 1-year [40, 44, 53, 54, 60, 61, 66]. In contrast, most (4 out of 7) studies indicated no difference between groups following intervention periods less than 1-year when national standardised tests were used as the outcome measure [27, 42, 64, 66]. However, standardised test scores significantly improved following a 1-year [51] and 3-year physically active lesson intervention [58]. These results were confirmed in the meta-analysis. When progress monitoring tools were used (4 studies) as the outcome measure, academic-related outcomes generally showed improvement (standardised mean difference = 1.03 (95% CI: -0.22,1.84)). However, when measured using a national standardised test (6 studies), academic-related outcomes generally showed no improvement (standardised mean difference = −1.13 (95% CI: -0.72,0.46)) (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on academic achievement

In addition to standardised tests and progress monitoring tools, a small number of studies (not included in the meta-analysis) measured academic achievement via grades, content recall quizzes and self-reported academic competence. Results were inconsistent. One study reported no difference between groups for grades across eight subjects (total score) following a 20-week active break program [47], Another reported a greater proportion of students in the control group showed improvement in grades for math and reading, compared with an active break intervention group [42]. Other studies assessed academic achievement via content recall quizzes and perceptions of academic competence, with no difference between groups in math and social studies scores following participation in single lessons lasting between 10 and 30 min [59, 63]. Another study reported self-reported perceptions of academic competence improved during physically active lessons [56].

Dose response relationship

Four studies aimed to explore the optimal dose of active break (i.e. amount of physical activity required to confer academic benefits) required to provide maximum effects on academic-related outcomes, by manipulating intensity [41], duration [39, 40], and frequency [49] of active break sessions. Howie and colleagues [39, 40] compared 5-, 10- and 20-min active breaks with a 10-min no break condition. Results showed on-task classroom behaviour significantly improved after the 10-min active break condition [39] and math scores were highest after the 10-min (ES = 0.24) and 20-min (ES = 0.27) active break conditions [40]. Janssen et al. [41] compared selective attention scores across 15 min of each of the following four conditions: no break (continued with school work), passive break (teacher read story), moderate-intensity active break (jogging, passing, dribbling), and vigorous-intensity active break (running, jumping, skipping) [41]. Results showed that selective attention scores improved most after the moderate-intensity active break [41]. Altenburg and colleagues [49] compared acute effects of different frequencies (one per day vs. twice per day) of 20 min moderate-intensity active breaks. Results showed significantly better selective attention scores for children who received the twice per day frequency [49].

Physical activity outcomes

Eleven studies examined the effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on children’s physical activity levels using a range of measures, including questionnaire [35], direct observation [45], pedometer [25, 47, 52], and accelerometer [27, 28, 36, 47, 58, 60, 63]. Across most (10 out of 11) classroom-based physical activity interventions, small increases in physical activity were reported [25, 27, 28, 35, 36, 45, 52, 58, 60, 63]. Across studies there was a 2% to 16% increase in moderate- to vigorous- intensity physical activity during intervention lessons, [27, 28, 45, 60, 63], and 2% to 12% increase in school day moderate- to vigorous- intensity physical activity [27, 28, 58]. However, as shown in Fig. 5 results from 3 studies included in meta-analysis indicate classroom-based physical activity did not affect physical activity (standardised mean difference = 0.40 (95% CI: -0.15,0.95).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Forrest plot of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on physical activity

Discussion

A systematic search of the literature found 39 studies assessing the effect of classroom-based physical activity on academic-related outcomes, including classroom behaviour, cognitive function and academic achievement. In the majority of studies, academic-related outcomes improved following participation in classroom-based physical activity programs. These findings are generally consistent with earlier reviews finding that overall physical activity level was either positively associated, or was not associated with academic-related outcomes [14, 15, 17]. In addition, the interventions included in the current review generally resulted in more physical activity.

The finding that classroom-based physical activity improves on-task or reduces off-task classroom behaviour immediately following participation in intervention sessions is consistent with previous reviews of school-based physical activity. For example, systematic reviews of the effect of physical activity during the school break time on academic-related outcomes showed positive associations between participation in physical activity before class (e.g. during recess/snack time) and on-task classroom behaviour in subsequent lessons [17, 29]. Therefore, breaking up lesson time with physical activity offers a promising strategy to improve on-task behaviour. Further, physically active lessons may provide a strategy to engage students in lesson content, which may lead to improved on-task classroom behaviour. However, this assumption is purely speculative and further research is needed to confirm this. One study reported a non-significant increase in on-task classroom behaviour after intervention sessions, compared with control [55]. A possible reason for this finding may be that the sample size in that study (n = 97) may not have been large enough to detect a significant improvement. Few studies (n = 3) reported that classroom-based physical activity had no effect on classroom behaviour. The majority of these studies (2 out of 3) reported that, while behaviour incidents and off-task behaviour increased in both the intervention and control groups, the increase was greater in the control group, compared with the intervention group [46, 47]. These findings may encourage teachers to consider implementing classroom-based physical activity programs by alleviating concerns about reducing on-task behaviour due to the disruption to the classroom routine [10].

While classroom-based physical activity showed relatively consistent positive associations with classroom behaviour, effects on cognitive function were inconsistent. A possible explanation for this finding may relate to the variability in the quality of measures used. Overall results showed studies that reported improvements in cognitive function used measures with moderate to high levels of reliability and validity [67, 68]. In contrast, studies reporting no improvement in cognitive function mainly used measures with lower levels of reliability and validity [69,70,71]. It may be important for future studies to use tests of cognitive function with established validity and reliability.

A further possible explanation for inconsistent effects on cognitive function may relate to the level of cognitive engagement inherent in each type of classroom-based physical activity. It has been suggested that cognitively engaging physical activity (i.e. physical activity combined with cognitive demands) may enhance cognitive function to a greater degree than non-cognitively engaging physical activity (e.g. repetitive exercise) [72]. As curriculum-focused active breaks and physically active lessons can be considered cognitively engaging physical activity, it could be hypothesised that these types of classroom-based physical activity would lead to greater improvements in cognitive function, compared with active breaks that involve no cognitive content. While the majority of physically active lesson and curriculum focussed active break interventions (2 out of 3 studies) and only half of active break interventions (5 out of 10 studies) led to improvements in cognitive function, there were too few cognitively engaging interventions included in the review to draw a definitive conclusion. The one study that compared cognitively engaging and non-cognitively engaging active breaks, showed an impact on cognitive outcomes for the cognitively engaging breaks group only, lending support to this hypothesis [26]. Although not explicitly stated, many studies which do not purport to involve cognitively engaging physical activity involve some activities which are likely to confer cognitive engagement e.g. hopping sequences to music [37, 38], and coordinative exercises [50]. Some of these report positive and some null findings, yet it is difficult to ascertain the proportion of physical activity children were exposed to that was cognitively engaging. Future studies are encouraged to separate the effects of cognitively engaging and non-cognitively engaging physical activity on cognitive functions.

In addition to the cognitive test used, results may be dependent on the type of cognitive function assessed. For example, classroom-based physical activity appeared to have a particularly beneficial effect on selective attention [24, 41, 49], compared with other components of cognitive function, including sustained attention [46], fluid intelligence [54, 64], information processing speed [50], and executive function [32, 37, 38, 40]. However, a recent systematic review concluded that there is insufficient evidence to conclude what specific cognitive functions are most affected by physical activity [73]. Exercise-induced arousal may provide a further explanation for inconsistency in findings. This theory suggests that the heightened level of arousal during physical activity facilitates cognitive function and that this effect may be moderated by physical activity intensity [74]. However, while the majority of included studies reported a target physical activity intensity, few measured physical activity intensity during interventions precluding conclusions regarding the role of physical activity intensity on cognitive function. Thus, the favourable effect of physical activity on selective attention indicated in this review requires further research for confirmation. Nonetheless, should improvements in selective attention occur, such as the ability to ignore distractions this may be of particular interest to teachers and may provide motivation to incorporate physical activity into their classroom routine.

In addition to classroom behaviour and cognitive function, classroom-based physical activity may also have a positive effect on academic achievement. However, effects on academic achievement may be dependent on intervention duration and the type of assessment tool used to measure academic achievement. In the current review it appeared that interventions of shorter duration were more likely to show an improvement in academic achievement if a progress monitoring tool was used, rather than a national standardised test. This may be because curriculum-based measures are sensitive to small changes in academic achievement, and can be administered frequently (e.g. weekly) [75, 76], while standardised tests are usually designed to be administered less frequently (e.g. yearly), and are not sensitive to short-term progress. Therefore, progress monitoring tools may be a more suitable choice to determine intervention effects on academic achievement in the short-term. This finding has important implications for future research, indicating it may be important to consider intervention duration when selecting the measure of academic achievement. Therefore, future intervention studies may consider using a progress monitoring tool for intervention periods less than 1-year, and standardised tests for intervention periods longer than 1-year if academic achievement is the outcome of interest.

Other studies investigated the impact of different doses of classroom-based physical activity on academic-related outcomes. However, results are based on few (n = 4) heterogeneous studies which considered a limited range of potential physical activity doses. Thus, further research is needed to be able to draw conclusions regarding the minimal dose of active break required to impact academic-related outcomes.

Several studies aimed to explore the effect of classroom-based physical activity on children’s physical activity levels [25, 27, 28, 35, 36, 45, 47, 52, 58, 60, 63]. Results from the meta-analysis showed classroom-based physical activity did not affect physical activity levels. However, as only three of the 11 identified studies could be included in the meta-analysis these results should be interpreted with caution, and further research is warranted. Findings from the systematic review consistently revealed small increases in physical activity in children participating in the intervention, compared with students in the comparison group. These findings are in line with results from another review reporting positive associations between classroom-based physical activity interventions and children’s physical activity levels [21]. While promising, it is possible compensation for this activity occurs outside of school. However, with limited information available, it is difficult to make strong conclusions on this. Further, it can be difficult to implement physical activity interventions in schools, often due to a lack of time associated with competing curriculum demands [77]. However, classroom-based physical activity is unique from other forms of school-based physical activity (e.g. Physical Education class and school sport) in that it does not compete for instructional time (physically active lessons and curriculum-focussed active breaks) or requires only minimal time commitment (active breaks). Thus, classroom-based physical activity may be a potentially appealing option for schools as it offers a time-efficient strategy to promote physical activity.

Limitations

The considerable variation between studies in study designs, intervention content and outcome assessment tools make it difficult to draw definitive conclusions, as evidenced by the small proportion of studies that could be included in meta-analyses. For studies that assessed intervention effects on physical activity, the majority compared physical activity levels during the classroom-based physical activity session, with a traditional seated lesson [27, 28, 45, 47], or assessed intervention effects on school day physical activity levels only [25, 27, 28, 36, 52, 60]. Therefore, it is unclear if the increase in physical activity during these sessions is compensated for by a reduction in physical activity at other times of the day. However, as intervention effects on improving on-task, reducing off-task classroom behaviour and cognitive function appear to be primarily acute, this may not be a problem for these outcomes. In addition, few studies used an objective measure of physical activity intensity [27, 28, 35, 36, 47, 58, 60, 63]. Thus, future studies using objective measures of physical activity are required to determine intervention effects on overall moderate- to- vigorous-intensity physical activity, and to determine intervention fidelity (i.e. if the required physical activity intensity is met) within the sessions. Lastly, given that the majority of included studies reported significant improvements in academic-related outcomes, it is possible publication bias may have impacted the lack of published null associations.

Conclusion

Classroom-based physical activity interventions may provide a practical, low-cost, and effective strategy to increase academic-related outcomes, particularly acute positive effects on improving on-task and reducing off-task classroom behaviour and selective attention. Classroom-based physical activity could also have the potential to increase children’s physical activity levels, however further research is needed to confirm this. Findings from this systematic review should be interpreted with caution given the high number of included studies of low methodological quality, suggesting there is room for improvement in classroom-based physical activity intervention study designs and reporting. This review has identified a number of areas for further research in order to increase understanding of the effect of classroom-based physical activity on academic and physical activity outcomes. These include the need for future studies to use objective measures of physical activity, and to consider intervention duration when selecting a measure of academic achievement. In addition, future studies should explore the effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on specific cognitive outcomes, as well as the impact of different types of physical activity (aerobic versus anaerobic versus resistance training and cognitively engaging vs. non-cognitively engaging physical activity) on academic-related outcomes. Further, it is not clear if improvements in academic-related outcomes are a result of the physical activity or a result of the break from academic instruction, therefore future research is encouraged to add an attention control group. Lastly, it is recommended future studies use a standardized measure of cognitive function with established reliability and validity to be able to make comparisons across studies.