Skip to main content
Log in

Neurostimulation Devices for Cognitive Enhancement: Toward a Comprehensive Regulatory Framework

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Neuroethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is mounting evidence that non-invasive brain stimulation devices - transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) could be used for cognitive enhancement. However, the regulatory environment surrounding such uses of stimulation devices is less clear than for stimulant drugs—a fact that has already been commercially exploited by several companies. In this paper, the mechanism of action, uses and adverse effects of non-invasive neurostimulation devices are reviewed, along with social and ethical challenges pertaining to their use as cognitive enhancements. Two regulatory approaches that could be used to facilitate responsible use of these devices as products and services are outlined. Apart from establishing the urgently needed comprehensive regulatory framework, they might provide a starting point for establishing long term physiological and social effects of enhancement uses of tDCS and TMS.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A notable example is: “Schoolchildren who struggle to grasp mathematics could benefit from having their brains roused with electricity” [27].

  2. The inhibitory effect of cathodal stimulation seems to be consistent in motor studies; in the cognitive category, memory and executive function are affected, whereas the evidence on effects on language ability is not conclusive (see [34]).

  3. The question of liability for harmful effects after the training has been provided is salient, but easily answered in this context. Just as driving instructors are not liable for the traffic accidents made by their trainees after they pass the exams and receive their licenses, so tDCS trainers should not be held responsible for adverse effects caused by “trained and certified” individuals. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for constructive comments that have prompted me to make this more clear.

  4. Luber & Lisanby [5] have reviewed evidence on three different kinds of enhancement effects, along with current hypotheses for explaining the phenomena: (i) enhancement via nonspecific effects of TMS; (ii) enhancement via “addition by subtraction”, and (iii) enhancement effects via direct TMS to specific task-related brain regions.

  5. Compared to tDCS, TMS is much more dangerous. The danger of causing seizures is present even in controlled laboratory settings, let alone in “at home” uses, which basically justifies a more restrictive approach. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for constructive comments that have prompted me to make this more clear.

  6. The reliance on professional guidelines is problematic in two separate respects: 1. Such guidelines do not exert sufficient regulatory control, and 2. The lack of state involvement exacerbates the problem of lack of control. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for the constructive comments that provided input for this clarification.

  7. This assertion might need to be qualified. Namely, not everyone will see the trade-offs between enhancement effects and potential side-effects of tDCS and TMS in the same light. As I argue elsewhere (see [40]), the lack of adequate information on long term effects and even short term benefits (the issue how laboratory findings of improvement in cognition relate to everyday performance is far from clear) complicates the comparison of evaluations of benefits, harms and positional (dis)advantages. Since they are at this point not quantifiable for, say, rational choice modeling, the assumption regarding offsetting the concerns of fairness could be provisionally accepted, until disputed by further evidence. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for constructive comments that have prompted me to make this more clear.

  8. This could raise an issue of fairness for people who would like to benefit from enhancement services, but are unwilling or unable to demonstrate sufficient understanding of such complicated issues such as brain regions, electrical and magnetic fields, etc. Now, whether such an issue would materialize or not is an open question, but the provision that EDM would need to be fine-tuned (perhaps by dividing the exam in the theoretical and practical part, with one being sufficient to benefit from the service and both being necessary for product purchase) could offset this concern. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for constructive comments that have prompted me to make this more clear.

References

  1. Maguire, G.Q., and E.M. McGee. 1999. Implantable brain chips? Time for debate. Hastings Center Report 29(1): 7–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. The European Group on Ethics in Sciences and New Technologies (EGE). 2005. Opinion on the ethical aspects of ICT implants in the human body (16 March 2005.). Opinion of the European Group on Ethics in Sciences and New Technologies to the European Commission: Vol. 20. Luxembourg: Office for Official Publ. of the European Communities.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Warwick, K. 2008. Cybernetic enhancements. In Reshaping the human condition: Exploring human enhancement, ed. L. Zonneveld, H. Dijstelbloem, and D. Ringoir, 123–131. The Hague: Rathenau Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Science and Technology Options Assessment (STOA). 2009. Human enhancement study. The Hague: Rathenau Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Luber, B., and S.H. Lisanby. 2013. Enhancement of human cognitive performance using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). NeuroImage. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.007. [Epub ahead of print] June 13th 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Dockery, C.A., R. Hueckel-Weng, N. Birbaumer, and C. Plewnia. 2009. Enhancement of planning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience 29(22): 7271–7277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Adee, S. 2012. Zap your brain into the zone: Fast track to pure focus, New Scientist 2850 http://www.newscientist.com/article/mg21328501.600-zap-your-brain-into-the-zone-fast-track-to-pure-focus.html?full=true Accessed on 23.03.2012.

  8. Dubljević, V., V. Saigle, and E. Racine. 2014. The rising tide of tDCS in the media and academic literature. Neuron 82(4): 731–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Clark, V.P., and R. Parasuraman. 2013. Neuroenhancement: Enhancing brain and mind in health and in disease. NeuroImage 85: 889–894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Fitz, N.S. & Reiner, P. B. 2013. The challenge of crafting policy for do-it-yourself brain stimulation. Journal of Medical Ethics, Epub ahead of print June 3, 2013 doi: 10.1136/medethics-2013-101458

  11. Anonymous. 2013. Brain blast: DIY attempts at electrical brain stimulation to improve cognition are to get easier, Nature 498: 271–272

  12. Bikson, M., S. Bestman, and S. Edwards. 2013. Neuroscience: Transcranial devices are not playthings. Nature 501: 167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Maslen, H., J. Savulescu, T. Douglas, N. Levy, and R. Cohen Kadosh. 2013. Regulation of devices for cognitive enhancement. The Lancet 382: 938–939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Pascual-Leone, A., F. Fregni, M. Steven-Wheeler, and L. Forrow. 2011. Non-invasive brain stimulation as a therapeutic and investigative tool: An ethical appraisal. In Oxford handbook of neuroethics, ed. J. Illes and B. Sahakian, 417–440. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Simpson, K.N., M.J. Welch, A.F. Kozel, M.A. Demitrack, and Z. Nahas. 2009. Cost-effectiveness of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of major depression: A health economics analysis. Advances in Therapy 26(3): 346–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Cohen Kadosh, R., N. Levy, J. O’Shea, et al. 2012. The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation. Current Biology 22(4): 108–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. United Nations (UN). 1971. Convention on Psychotropic Substances. www.unodc.org/pdf/convention_1971_en.pdf.

  18. Fregni, F. 2005. Transcranial direct current stimulation. British Journal of Psychiatry 186: 446–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Brunoni, A.R., et al. 2011. Clinical research with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): Challenges and future directions. Brain Stimulation. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.002.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Clark, V.P., et al. 2012. TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to identify concealed objects. NeuroImage 59: 117–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Priori, A. 2003. Brain polarization in humans: A reappraisal of an old tool for prolonged non-invasive modulation of brain excitability. Clinical Neurophysiology 114: 589–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Kincses, T.Z., et al. 2003. Facilitation of probabilistic classification learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the human. Neuropsychologia 42: 113–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Antal, A., et al. 2004. Facilitation of visuo-motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor and extrastriate visual areas in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience 19: 2888–2892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Dockery, C.A., D. Liebetanz, N. Birbaumer, M. Malinowska, and M.J. Wesierska. 2011. Cumulative benefits of frontal transcranial direct current stimulation on visuospatial working memory training and skill learning in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 96(3): 452–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Hamilton, R., et al. 2011. Rethinking the thinking cap: Ethics of neural enhancement using noninvasive brain stimulation. Neurology 76(2): 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Cohen Kadosh, R., et al. 2010. Modulating neuronal activity produces specific and long-lasting changes in numerical competence. Current Biology 20: 2016–2020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Sample, I. 2010. Electrical stimulation of the brain boosts math skills, claim scientists, The Guardian, November 4th, 2010

  28. Poreisz, C., et al. 2007. Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Research Bulletin 72: 208–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Riedel, P., et al. 2011. Contact dermatitis after transcranial direct current stimulation. Brain Stimulation 5(3): 432–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Arul-Anandam, A.P., C. Loo, and P. Mitchell. 2010. Induction of hypomanic episode with transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Electro-Convulsive Therapy 26(1): 68–69.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Galvez, V., et al. 2011. Hypomania induction in a patient with bipolar II disorder by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Journal of Electro-Convulsive Therapy 27: 256–258.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Brunoni, A.R., et al. 2011. Manic psychosis after sertraline and transcranial direct-current stimulation. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 22(3): e4–e5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Iuculcano, T., and R. Cohen Kadosh. 2013. The mental cost of cognitive enhancement. The Journal of Neuroscience 33(10): 4482–4486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Jacobson, L., M. Koslowski, and M. Lavidor. 2012. tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: A meta-analytical review. Experimental Brain Research 216: 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Stagg, C.J., and M.A. Nitsche. 2011. Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. The Neuroscientist 17(1): 37–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Maslen, H., T. Douglas, R. Cohen Kadosh, N. Levy, and J. Savulescu. 2014. The regulation of cognitive enhancement devices: Extending the medical model. Journal of Law and the Biosciences 1(1): 68–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. English, V. 2007. Boosting your brainpower: ethical aspects of cognitive enhancements. A discussion paper from the British Medical Association. London: British Medical Association.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Dubljević, V. 2012. Toward a legitimate public policy on cognition-enhancement drugs. American Journal of Bioethics – Neuroscience 3(3): 29–33.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Dubljević, V. 2013. Prohibition or coffee-shops: Regulation of amphetamine and methylphenidate for enhancement use by healthy adults. American Journal of Bioethics 13(7): 23–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Dubljević, V. 2013. Cognitive enhancement, rational choice and justification. Neuroethics 6(1): 179–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Larriviere, D., M.A. Williams, M. Rizzo, and R.J. Bonnie. 2009. Responding to requests from adult patients for neuroenhancements: Guidance of the ethics, law and humanities committee. Neurology 73(17): 1406–1412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Sutherland, S. 2013. Magnetic brain stimulation could ease pain, Scientific American, 17.03.2013. available at http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=magnetic-brain-stimulation-could-ease-pain accessed on December 15th 2013.

  43. Fitzgerald, P.B., and Z.J. Daskalakis. 2013. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for depressive disorders: A practical guide. Dodrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  44. Schermer, M. 2014. Reducing, restoring or enhancing autonomy with neuromodulation techniques. In Free will and the brain: Neuroscientific, philosophical, and legal perspectives, ed. W. Glannon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Richter, L. 2013. Robotized transcranial magnetic stimulation. Dodrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  46. Rossi, S., M. Hallett, P.M. Rossini, et al. 2009. Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clinical Neurophysiology 120(12): 2008–2039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Boniface, S., and U. Ziemann (eds.). 2003. Plasticity in the human nervous system: Investigations with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Bersani, F.S., A. Minichino, P.G. Enticott, et al. 2013. Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation as a treatment for psychiatric disorders: A comprehensive review. European Psychiatry 28: 30–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Rothwell, J.C. 2003. Techniques of transcranial magnetic simulation. In Plasticity in the human nervous system: Investigations with transcranial magnetic stimulation, ed. S. Boniface and U. Ziemann, 26–61. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Rioult-Pedotti, M.S., and J.P. Donoghue. 2003. The nature and mechanisms of plasticity. In Plasticity in the human nervous system: Investigations with transcranial magnetic stimulation, ed. S. Boniface and U. Ziemann, 1–25. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  51. McKinley, R.A., N. Bridges, C.M. Walters, and J. Nelson. 2012. Modulating the brain at work using noninvasive transcranial stimulation. NeuroImage 59: 129–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Ranisch, R., D. Garofoli, and V. Dubljević. 2013. ‘Clock shock’, motivational enhancement and performance maintenance in Adderall use. American Journal of Bioethics – Neuroscience 4(1): 13–14.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Luber, B., J. Steffener, A. Tucker, C. Habeck, A.V. Peterchev, Z.D. Deng, R.C. Basner, Y. Stern, and S.H. Lisanby. 2013. Extended remediation of sleep deprived-induced working memory deficits using fMRI-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation. Sleep 36(6): 857–871.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Boroojerdi, B., M. Phipps, L. Kopylev, C.M. Wharton, L.G. Cohen, and J. Grafman. 2001. Enhancing analogic reasoning with rTMS over the left prefrontal cortex. Neurology 56: 526–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Snyder, A., H. Bahramali, T. Hawker, and D.J. Mitchell. 2006. Savant-like numerosity skills in normal people by magnetic pulses. Perception 35: 837–845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Snyder, A. 2009. Explaining and inducing savant skills: Privileged access to lower level, less-processed information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Britain 364: 1399–1405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Keck, M.E., I. Sillaber, K. Ebner, et al. 2000. Acute transcranial magnetic stimulation of frontal brain regions selectively modulates the release of vasopressin, biogenic amines and amino acids in the rat brain. European Journal of Neuroscience 12: 3713–3720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Illes, J., and B. Sahakian (eds.). 2011. Oxford handbook of neuroethics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Patel, P., C.C. Meltzer, H.S. Mayberg, and K. Levine. 2007. The role of imaging in United States courtrooms. Neuroimaging Clinics of North America 17(4): 557–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Sandberg, A., W. Sinnott-Armstrong, and J. Savulescu. 2011. Cognitive enhancements in court. In The Oxford handbook of neuroethics, ed. J. Illes and B. Sahakian, 273–284. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Blank, R.H. 2013. Intervention in the brain: Politics, policy and ethics. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Bostrom, N., and A. Sandberg. 2009. Cognitive enhancement: Methods, ethics, regulatory challenges. Science and Engineering Ethics 15: 311–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Veljko Dubljević.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dubljević, V. Neurostimulation Devices for Cognitive Enhancement: Toward a Comprehensive Regulatory Framework. Neuroethics 8, 115–126 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-014-9225-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-014-9225-0

Keywords

Navigation