Skip to main content
Log in

Welche Rolle spielt neben Merkmalen des Instruktionsdesigns die fachspezifische und aufgabenspezifische Motivation beim Lernen mit Multimedia im naturwissenschaftlichen Unterricht?

What is the role of domain and task-specific motivation alongside instructional design for learning with multimedia in the science classroom?

  • Schwerpunkt
  • Published:
Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Zusammenfassung

In diesem Beitrag wird eine empirische Studie1 zum Lernen mit Texten und Animationen im naturwissenschaftlichen Unterricht vorgestellt. In einem 2 × 2-Design wurden die Verfügbarkeit multimedialen Lernmaterials (nur Text vs. Text + Animationen) und die Modalität des erläuternden Textes (geschrieben vs. gesprochen) variiert. Als kontinuierlicher Faktor wurde vor Beginn der Lernphase die Motivation der Lernenden erfasst. Unmittelbar nach der Lernphase wurden die Erinnerungs- und Transferleistungen bestimmt. Die Ergebnisse zeigen bessere Erinnerungsleistungen für Lernende mit multimedialem Lernmaterial, vorausgesetzt, dass die Animationen von gesprochenem Text begleitet wurden. Allerdings war der beobachtbare Multimediaeffekt im Vergleich zu den positiven Effekten der fachspezifischen Lernmotivation nicht für Transfer beobachtbar. Die Effekte des Multimediadesigns traten unabhängig von der Lernmotivation auf. Implikationen für Folgestudien werden diskutiert.

Abstract

The present paper reports on an empirical study which investigated learning with text and animations in the science classroom. In a 2 × 2 design the presence of multimedia learning material instruction (text only vs. text + animations) as well as the modality of the explanatory text (spoken vs. written) were tested. Prior to learning, students’ motivation to learn was assessed as a continuous factor. Recall and transfer were assessed immediately after learning. The results show better recall of information for learners with multimedia materials, providing the animations were accompanied by spoken text. However, in contrast to the positive effects of domain-specific motivation this multimedia effect was not evident for transfer. The effects of multimedia design were independent of motivation. Implications for future studies are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Abb. 1

Notes

  1. Die Studie wurde durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft im Rahmen eines Projekts der Forschergruppe „Orchestrierung computergestützter Lehr-Lernprozesse“ gefördert (GE992/5-1).

  2. Die hier berichteten Daten beruhen auf Reanalysen eines Teildatensatz einer umfangreicheren Studie, von der andere Teildatensätze unter anderen Fragestellungen an anderer Stelle publiziert wurden (Scheiter et al. 2014).

  3. Gegenüber der Originalversion des Tests wurde in der vorliegenden Untersuchung die Skala „Angst vor Misserfolg“ nicht berücksichtigt.

Literatur

  • Anglin, G., Vaez, H., & Cunningham, K. (2004). Visual representations and learning: The role of static and animated graphics. In D. Jonassen (Hrsg.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (2. Aufl., S. 865–916). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Koning, B. B., Tabbers, H. K., Rikers, R. M. J. P., & Paas, F. (2009). Towards a framework for attention cueing in instructional animations: Guidelines for research and design. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 113–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B., & Poyas, Y. (2008). Learning with multiple representations: Extending multimedia learning beyond the lab. Learning and Instruction, 18, 368–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flores, F., Tovar, M. E., & Gallegos, L. (2003). Representation of the cell and its processes in high school students: An integrated view. International Journal of Science Education, 25, 269–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, A., Taskinen, P., Schütte, K., Prenzel, M., Artelt, C., Baumert, J., Blum, W., Hammann, M., Klieme, E., & Pekrun, R. (Hrsg.) (2009). PISA 2006 Skalenhandbuch – Dokumentation der Erhebungsinstrumente. Münster: Waxmann.

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 69–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. In M. P. Zanna (Hrsg.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Bd. 38, S. 69–119). San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., Jamet, E., & Dubois, V. (2008). The role of working memory components in multimedia comprehension. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 353–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., & Tardieu, H. (1999). The role of illustrations in text comprehension: what, when, for whom, and why? In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Hrsg.), The construction of mental representations during reading (S. 195–218). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harskamp, G. E., Mayer R. E., & Suhre, C. (2007). Does the modality principle for multimedia learning apply to science classrooms? Learning and Instruction, 17, 465–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., & Just, M. A. (1993). Constructing mental models of machines from text and diagrams. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 717–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helmke, A. (2009). Unterrichtsqualität und Lehrerprofessionalität. Diagnose, Evaluation und Verbesserung des Unterrichts. Seelze: Klett-Kallmeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Höffler, T., & Leutner, D. (2007). Instructional animation versus static pictures: A meta-analysis. Learning and Instruction, 17, 722–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imhof, B., Scheiter, K., & Gerjets, P. (2011). Learning about locomotion patterns from visualizations: Effects of presentation format and realism. Computers & Education, 57, 1961–1970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imhof, B., Scheiter, K., Edelmann, J., & Gerjets, P. (2012). How temporal and spatial aspects of presenting visualizations affect learning about locomotion patterns. Learning and Instruction, 22, 193–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, A. M., Azevedo, R., & D’Mello, S. K. (2011). The temporal and dynamic nature of self-regulatory processes during independent and externally assisted hypermedia learning. Cognition and Instruction, 29, 471–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kombartzky, U., Ploetzner, R., Schlag, S., & Metz, B. (2010). Developing and evaluating a strategy for learning from animations. Learning and Instruction, 20, 424–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kühl, T., Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Edelmann, J. (2011a). The influence of text modality on learning with static and dynamic visualizations. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 29–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kühl, T., Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Gemballa, S. (2011b). Can differences in learning strategies explain the benefits of learning from static and dynamic visualizations? Computers & Education, 56, 176–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larkin J., & Simon H. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, W., & Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory, modality of presentation and the transient information effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 943–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. R., Anglin, G. J., & Carney, R. N. (1987). On empirically validating functions of pictures in prose. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Hrsg.), The Psychology of Illustration: I. Basic Research (S. 51–85). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J., Leach, J., & Wood-Robinson, C. (2000). Chromosomes: The missing link – young people’s understanding of mitosis, meiosis, and fertilisation. Journal of Biological Education, 34, 189–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R. K. (2003). Animation and learning: Selective processing of information in dynamic graphics. Learning and Instruction, 13, 157–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, B. L., Newhouse, P., Pagram, J., Campbell, A., & Schulz, H. (2002). A comparison of temporal speech and text cueing in educational multimedia. Journal of Computer-Assisted Learning, 18, 296–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2. Aufl.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., Levinstein, I. B., & Boonthum, C. (2004). iStart: Interactive strategy trainer for active reading and thinking. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 222–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. (2009). Toward a Comprehensive Model of Comprehension. In B. Ross (Hrsg.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Bd. 51, S. 297–384). Burlington: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McTigue, E. M. (2009). Does multimedia learning theory extend to middle-school students? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 143–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., Ozogul, G., & Reisslein, M. (2011). Teaching with concrete and abstract visual representations: Effects on students’ problem solving, problem representations, and learning perceptions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 32–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R. (1999). The role of motivation in promoting and sustaining self-regulated learning. International Journal of Educational Research, 31, 459–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., & Burns, B. D. (2001). FAM: Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung aktueller Motivation in Lern- und Leistungssituationen. Diagnostica, 47, 57–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L. P. (1990). Using computer animated graphics in science instruction with children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 135–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riemeier, T. (2005). Schülervorstellungen von Zellen, Teilung und Wachstum. Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Naturwissenschaften, 11, 57–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rummer, R., Schweppe, J., Fürstenberg, A., Scheiter, K., & Zindler, A. (2011). The perceptual basis of the modality effect in multimedia learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 159–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., Huk, T., Imhof, B., & Kammerer, Y. (2009). The effects of realism in learning with dynamic visualizations. Learning and Instruction, 19, 481–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheiter, K., Schüler, A., Gerjets, P., Huk, T., & Hesse, F. W. (2014). Extending multimedia research: How do prerequisite knowledge and reading comprehension affect learning from text and pictures. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 73–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheiter, K., Schubert, C., Gerjets, P., & Stalbovs, K. (im Druck). Does a strategy training foster students’ ability to learn from multimedia? The Journal of Experimental Education.

  • Scheiter, K., Wiebe, E., & Holsanova, J. (2008). Theoretical and instructional aspects of learning with visualizations. In R. Zheng (Hrsg.), Cognitive effects of multimedia learning (S. 67–88). Hershey: IGI Global.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., & Schreyer, I. (1994). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Lernen. Ein Überblick zu Ergebnissen der Forschung. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 8, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., Streblow, L., Ermgassen, U., & Moschner, B. (2003). Lernmotivation und Lernstrategien als Bedingungen der Studienleistung. Ergebnisse einer Längsschnittstudie. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 17, 185–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, S., Wild, K., & Winteler, A. (1995). Lernaufwand und Elaborationsstrategien als Mediatoren der Beziehung von Studieninteresse und Studienleistung. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 9, 181–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt-Weigand, F., Kohnert, A., & Glowalla, U. (2010). Explaining the modality and contiguity effects: New insights from investigating students’ viewing behaviour. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 226–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W. (2011). Colorful bouquets in multimedia research: A closer look at the modality effect. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 25, 269–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, A., Scheiter, K., Rummer, R., & Gerjets, P. (2012). Explaining the modality effect in multimedia learning: Is it due to a lack of temporal contiguity with written text and pictures? Learning and Instruction, 22, 92–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, A., Scheiter, K., & Schmidt-Weigand, F. (2011). Boundary conditions and constraints of the modality effect. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 25, 211–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L. (1995). Reasoning about the referent of a picture versus reasoning about the picture as a referent: an effect of visual realism. Memory & Cognition, 23, 709–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segers, E., Verhoeven, L., & Hulstijn-Hendrikse, N. (2008). Cognitive processes in children’s multimedia text learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 375–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stalbovs, K., Scheiter, K., & Gerjets, P. (eingereichtes Manuskript). Implementation intentions during multimedia learning: Using if-then plans to facilitate cognitive processing.

  • Stiller, K. D., Freitag, A., Zinnbauer, P., & Freitag, C. (2009). How pacing of multimedia instructions can influence modality effects: A case of superiority of visual texts. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 25, 184–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabbers, H. K., Martens, R. L., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Multimedia instructions and cognitive load theory: Effects of modality and cueing. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 71–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M., van Hout-Wolters, B., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vollmeyer, R., & Rheinberg, F. (1998). Motivationale Einflüsse auf Erwerb und Anwendung von Wissen in einem computersimulierten System. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 11–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetzel, C. D., Radtke, P. H., & Stern, H. W. (1994). Instructional effectiveness of video media. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witteman, M. J., & Segers, E. (2010). The modality effect tested in children in a user-paced multimedia environment. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 26, 132–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, A., Leahy, W., Marcus, N., & Sweller, J. (2012). Cognitive load theory, the transient information effect, and e-learning. Learning and Instruction, 22, 449–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katharina Scheiter.

Elektronisches zusätzliches Material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Scheiter, K., Schüler, A., Gerjets, P. et al. Welche Rolle spielt neben Merkmalen des Instruktionsdesigns die fachspezifische und aufgabenspezifische Motivation beim Lernen mit Multimedia im naturwissenschaftlichen Unterricht?. Z Erziehungswiss 17, 279–296 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-014-0516-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11618-014-0516-3

Schlüsselwörter

Keywords

Navigation