Skip to main content
Log in

The efficacy of an immersive 3D virtual versus 2D web environment in intercultural sensitivity acquisition

  • Cultural and Regional Perspectives
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Relatively few studies have empirically tested computer-based immersive virtual environments’ efficacy in teaching or enhancing pro-social attitudes, such as intercultural sensitivity. This channel study experiment was conducted (N = 159) to compare what effects, if any, an immersive 3D virtual environment would have upon subjects’ intercultural sensitivity, compared to a 2D web environment. Significant gains in intercultural sensitivity were found, with subjects exposed to the Second Life-based immersive virtual environment scoring higher than web-based subjects, both toward other cultures in general and toward Chinese culture in particular. An interaction effect for gender and channel was found, with males experiencing greater intercultural sensitivity outcomes than in a web environment. These empirical findings add to our theoretical understanding of channel effects and intercultural sensitivity. The results can guide cultural instructors or trainers toward the best platforms for content delivery to learners. These findings are worthy of future investigation to better understand their application in educational endeavors as well as in other fields.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. On an administered demographic questionnaire, participants self-identified as male or female.

  2. The decision to use China as the exemplar was made in large part due to the many observable cultural differences with the Western world, which provide sufficient distinctions for experimental testing and aided content and stimuli development for testing intercultural sensitivity outcomes. China's economic and cultural influences upon the Western world make this choice increasingly relevant. Future experiments may wish to test other cultural stimuli.

  3. In addition to the randomized assignment of conditions, the study was approved by a university IRB and no concerns were raised with the design, ethical or otherwise, thus we feel confident that ethical research practices were followed here.

  4. Such an acclimation or “practice” procedure is common in experiments to make sure users have a “test run.” It is also common when introducing users to new technologies. For example, Second Life recommends its users all begin at “Orientation Island,” a practice setting where users can learn how to navigate their avatars and familiarize themselves with basic commands and communications used within the environment before entering other places within Second Life.

  5. It should be noted that independent sample t tests comparing intercultural sensitivity mean scores between channels (Web and Second Life) at the pre-test level revealed no significant differences, providing a good baseline condition for the testing that followed. This baseline condition held both for intercultural sensitivity “toward other cultures in general” and toward “Chinese culture specifically.”

  6. As further explanation for the non-significant difference between males’ and females’ intercultural sensitivity scores at all three points in time, there were different starting mean scores. Thus, males still revealed significant increases in their own Second Life outcomes (when comparing to their own starting levels of intercultural sensitivity). See Fig. 3.

  7. The immediate post-test result was non-significant for males, but the other two points in time had significantly higher scores for Second Life compared to the web.

  8. It is important to note that both females and males gained in intercultural sensitivity following exposure to the cultural stimuli according to gender-specific pre-test and post-test scores, in both the 2D web and 3D virtual environments. However, the focus of the study has been on comparative differences by channel and by gender.

References

  • Ahn, S. J., Le, A. M. T., & Bailenson, J. (2013). The effect of embodied experiences on self-other merging, attitude, and helping behavior. Media Psychology, 16, 7–38. doi:10.1080/15213269.2012.75587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson, J., Beall, A., Loomis, J., Blascovich, J., & Turk, M. (2004). Decoupling social interaction from behavior and form in collaborative virtual environments. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments, 13(4), 428–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson, J., Beall, A., Loomis, J., Blascovich, J., & Turk, M. (2005). Transformed social interaction, augmented gaze, and social influence in immersive virtual environments. Human Communication Research, 31(4), 511–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, D. S., Fonarow, G. C., Hays, R. D., & Mangione, C. M. (2000). Self-study from web-based and printed guideline materials: A randomized, controlled trial among resident physicians. Annals of Internal Medicine, 132(12), 938–946.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychologica, 129(3), 387–398. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouta, H., & Paraskeva, F. (2013). The cognitive apprenticeship theory for the teaching of mathematics in an online 3D virtual environment. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science & Technology, 44(2), 159–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bratanova, B., & Kashima, Y. (2014). The “saying is repeating” effect: Dyadic communication can generate cultural stereotypes. Journal of Social Psychology, 154(2), 155–174. doi:10.1080/00224545.2013.874326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brislin, R. W., Cushner, K., Cherrie, C., & Yong, M. (1986). Intercultural interactions: A practical guide. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butter, M. C., Valenzuela, M. S., & Quintana, M. G. B. (2015). Intercultural talent management model: Virtual communities to promote collaborative learning in indigenous contexts. Teachers’ and students’ perceptions. Computers in Human Behavior. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain Miller, C. (2009, October 22). A virtual clinic to treat the stresses of war. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://bits.blogs.nytimes.com/2009/10/22/a-virtual-clinic-to-treat-the-stresses-of-war/.

  • Castel, A. D., Pratt, J., & Drummond, E. (2005). The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. Acta Psychologica, 119(2), 217–230. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, G.-M., & Starosta, W. J. (1996). Intercultural communication competence: A synthesis. In B. R. Burleson (Ed.), Communication yearbook (Vol. 19, pp. 353–384). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, G.-M., & Starosta, W. J. (1998). Foundations of intercultural communication. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, G.-M., & Starosta, W. J. (2000). The development and validation of the Intercultural Sensitivity Scale. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association. Seattle, WA.

  • Chia, N. K. H., & Li, J. (2012). Design of a generic questionnaire for reflective evaluation of a virtual reality-based intervention using virtual dolphins for children with autism. International Journal of Special Education, 27(3), 45–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coffey, A. J., Kamhawi, R., Fishwick, P., & Henderson, J. (2013). New media environments’ comparative effects upon intercultural sensitivity: A five-dimensional analysis. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 37, 605–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Zmigrod, S., & Hommel, B. (2013). Action video gaming and cognitive control: Playing first person shooter games is associated with improvement in working memory but not action inhibition. Psychological Research, 77(2), 234–239. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0415-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantine, M. G. (2000). Social desirability attitudes, sex, and affective and cognitive empathy as predictors of self-reported multicultural counseling competence. The Counseling Psychologist, 28(6), 857–872. doi:10.1177/0011000000286008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, G., & Khatchadourian, D. (2003). Empathy, ways of knowing, and interdependence as mediators of gender differences in attitudes toward hate speech and freedom of speech. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 300–308. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cundiff, N. L., & Komarraju, M. (2008). Gender differences in ethnocultural empathy and attitudes toward men and women in authority. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15(1), 5–15. doi:10.1037/t00520-000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cundiff, N. L., Nadler, J. T., & Swan, A. (2009). The influence of cultural empathy and gender on perceptions of diversity programs. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies., 16(1), 97–110. doi:10.1037/t00520-000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Castell, S., Jenson, J., & Larios, H. (2015). Gaming experience and spatial learning in a virtual Morris Water Maze. Virtual Worlds Research, 8(1), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deladisma, A., Cohen, M., Stevens, A., Wagner, P., Lok, B., Bernard, T., et al. (2007). Do medical students respond empathetically to a virtual patient? American Journal of Surgery, 193(6), 756–760. doi:10.1016/j.amjsurg.2007.01.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devita, G. (2000). Inclusive approaches to effective communication and active participation in the multicultural classroom: An international business management context. Active Learning in Higher Education, 1, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, W. C., & Prins, E. (2008). Unintended outcomes in Second Life: Intercultural literacy and cultural identity in a virtual world. Language and Intercultural Communication, 8(2), 101–118. doi:10.1080/14708470802139619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dong, Q., Day, K. D., & Collaço, C. M. (2008). Overcoming ethnocentrism through developing intercultural communication sensitivity and multiculturalism. Human Communication, 11(1), 27–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwardson, M., Kent, K., & McConnell, M. (1985). Television news information gain: Videotex versus a talking head. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 29(4), 367–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etengoff, A. (2008, December 29). Soldiers get virtual reality training. IT Examiner.com. Retrieved from http://www.itexaminer.com/soldiers-get-virtual-reality-training.aspx.

  • Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01990.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishwick, P., Henderson, J., Fresh, E., Futterknecht, F., & Hamilton, B. (2008). Simulating culture: An experiment using a multi-user virtual environment. In S. J. Mason, R. R. Hill, L. Mönch, O. Rose, T. Jefferson, & J. W. Fowler (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2008 Winter Simulation Conference (pp. 786–794). Piscataway, NJ: Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers, Inc.

  • Fox, J., & Bailenson, J. (2009). Virtual self-modeling: The effects of vicarious reinforcement and identification on exercise behaviors. Media Psychology, 12, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, W., Graf, A., Hentze, J., Mollenberg, A., & Chen, G. M. (2005). An examination of Chen and Starosta’s model of intercultural sensitivity in Germany and the United States. Intercultural Communication Studies, 14(1), 53–65. doi:10.1080/17475759.2010.534860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, W., Möllenberg, A., & Chen, G.-M. (2001). Measuring intercultural sensitivity in different cultural context. Paper presented at the biannual meeting of the International Association for Intercultural Communication Studies. Hong Kong.

  • Gega, L., White, R., Clarke, T., Turner, R., & Fowler, D. (2013). Virtual environments using video capture for social phobia with psychosis. Cyberpsychology, Behavior & Social Networking, 16(6), 473–479. doi:10.1089/cyber.2013.1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillath, O., McCall, C., Shaver, P. R., & Blascovich, J. (2008). What can virtual reality tell us about prosocial tendencies in real and virtual environments? Media Psychology, 11, 259–282. doi:10.1037/t01093-000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grabe, M., & Kamhawi, R. (2006). Hard wired for negative news? Gender differences in processing broadcast news. Communication Research, 33, 346–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature, 423(6939), 534–537. doi:10.1038/nature01647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M., Brannon, C., & Lohr, D. (1994). Two-dimensional representation of movement through three-dimensional space: The role of video game expertise. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15(1), 87–103. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(94)90007-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guadagno, R., Muscanell, N., Okdie, B., Burke, N., & Ward, T. (2011). Even in virtual environments women shop and men build: A social role perspective in Second Life. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 304–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, M., Ludlow, B., & Duff, M. (2015). Second Life®: A 3D virtual immersive environment for teacher preparation courses in a distance education program. Rural Special Education Quarterly, 34(3), 21–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J., Fishwick, P., Fresh, E., Futterknecht, F., & Hamilton, B. (2008). An immersive learning simulation environment for Chinese culture. In Proceedings of the Interservice/Industry Training, Simulation, and Education Conference (IITSEC) Orlando, Florida, paper 8344 (pp. 1–12).

  • Hodges, L. F., Rothbaum, B. O., Kooper, R., Meyer, T. C., Opdyke, D., de Graaff, J. J., et al. (1995). Virtual environments for treating the fear of heights. IEEE Computer, 28, 27–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holm, K., Nokelainen, P., & Tirri, K. (2009). Relationship of gender and academic achievement to Finnish students’ intercultural sensitivity. High Ability Studies, 20(2), 187–200. doi:10.1080/13598130903358543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holtz, S. (2007). Another world. Communication World, 243(May/June), 16–19.

  • Hunzaker, M. B. F. (2014). Making sense of misfortune: Cultural schemas, victim redefinition, and the perpetuation of stereotypes. Social Psychology Quarterly, 77(2), 166–184. doi:10.1177/0190272514521219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jain, S. (2013). Experiential training for enhancing intercultural sensitivity. Journal of Cultural Diversity, 20(1), 15–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jäncke, L., & Klimmt, C. (2011). Expertise in video gaming and driving skills. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 22(4), 279–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, G. (2007, March 12). A whole new virtual world: Media companies make 3D connections on Second Life and beyond. Broadcasting and Cable, p. 12.

  • Kamhawi, R., & Grabe, M. (2007). Why women are not watching: Gender differences in responding to negative, positive, and valence-ambiguous TV news. Paper presented to the meeting of the International Communication Association. San Francisco, CA.

  • Kamhawi, R., & Grabe, M. (2008). Engaging the female audience: An evolutionary psychology perspective on gendered responses to news valence frames. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(1), 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaupins, G. (2002). Trainer opinions of selected computer-based training methods. Journal of Education for Business, 77(July/August), 319–323.

  • Kazakova, S., Cauberghe, V., Pandalaere, M., & De Pelsmacker, P. (2014). Players’ expertise and competition with others shape the satisfaction of competence needs, gaming gratifications, and contingent self-esteem in a gaming context. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(1), 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2007). Gender differences in selective media use for mood management and mood adjustment. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 51(1), 73–92. doi:10.1080/08838150701308069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Alter, S. (2006). Mood adjustment to social situations through mass media use: How men ruminate and women dissipate angry moods. Human Communication Research, 32, 58–73. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2006.00003.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knobloch-Westerwick, S., Brück, J., & Hastall, M. R. (2006). The gender news use divide: Impacts of sex, gender, self-esteem, achievement and affiliation motivations on German news readers’ exposure to news topics. Communications—The European Journal of Communication, 31, 329–345. doi:10.1515/COMMUN.2006.021.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, T. (2010). Bridging social capital in online communities: Heterogeneity and social tolerance of online game players in Japan. Human Communication Research, 36, 546–569. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01388.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landis, D., & Bhagat, R. S. (1996). Handbook of intercultural training (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, H. C. (2009). Promoting metacognition in immersive cultural learning environments. In J. A. Jacko (Ed.), Human-computer interaction, Part IV, HCII, 2009. LNCS 5613 (pp. 129–139). Berlin: Springer. http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007%2F978-3-642-02583-9_15.

  • Lane, H. C., Hays, M. J., Core, M., & Auerbach, D. (2013). Learning intercultural communication skills with virtual humans: Feedback and fidelity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(4), 1026–1035. doi:10.1037/a0031506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30(11/12), 765–779. doi:10.1007/BF01544230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K. M., & Peng, W. (2006). What do we know about social and psychological effects of computer games? A comprehensive review of the current literature. In P. Vorderer & J. Bryant (Eds.), Playing video games: Motives, responses and consequences (pp. 325–346). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D. A. (1997). Interactive video games for health promotion: Effects on knowledge, self-efficacy, social support, and health. In R. L. Street Jr. & W. R. Gold (Eds.), Health promotion and interactive technology: Theoretical applications and future directions (pp. 103–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D. A. (2006). What can we learn from playing interactive games? In P. Vorderer & J. Bryant (Eds.), Playing video games: Motives, responses and consequences (pp. 379–398). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lim, S., & Reeves, B. (2009). Being in the game: Effects of avatar choice and point of view on psychophysiological responses during play. Media Psychology, 12, 348–370. doi:10.1080/15213260903287242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lull, R., & Bushman, B. (2016). Immersed in violence: Presence mediates the effect of 3D violent video gameplay on angry feelings. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5(2), 133–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandel, D. (2001). Muslims on the silver screen. Middle East Quarterly, 8(2), 19–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mantovani, F., Castelnuovo, G., Gaggioli, A., & Riva, G. (2003). Avatars in clinical psychology: A framework for the clinical use of virtual humans. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 6(2), 117–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mausehund, J. A., Timm, S. A., & King, A. S. (1995). Diversity training: Effects of an intervention treatment on nonverbal awareness. Business Communication Quarterly, 58(1), 27–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, D., & Chen, S. (2006). Serious games: Games that education, train, and inform. Boston: Thompson Course Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mozilla Foundation. (2016, March 2). What is the difference between webpage, website, web server, and search engine? Retrieved from https://developer.mozilla.org/en-US/docs/Learn/Common_questions/Pages_sites_servers_and_search_engines.

  • Nieto, C., & Booth, M. Z. (2010). Cultural competence: Its influence on the teaching and learning of international students. Journal of Studies in International Education, 14(4), 406–425. doi:10.1177/1028315309337929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. G., & Levy, R. M. (2008). Exploration through virtual reality: Encounters with the target culture. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 64(4), 663–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oei, A. C., & Patterson, M. D. (2013). Enhancing cognition with video games: A multiple game training study. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e58546. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orman, E. K. (2004). Effect of virtual reality graded exposure on anxiety levels of performing musicians: A case study. Journal of Music Therapy, 41(1), 70–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pazzaglia, F., & Cornoldi, C. (1999). The role of distinct components of visual-spatial working memory in the processing of texts. Memory, 7(1), 19–41. doi:10.1080/096582199388057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellas, N., & Kazanidis, I. (2014). Engaging students in blended and online collaborative courses at university level through Second Life: Comparative perspectives and instructional affordances. New Review of Hypermedia & Multimedia, 20(2), 123–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, S.-Y. (2006). A comparative perspective of intercultural sensitivity between college students and multinational employees in China. Multicultural Perspectives, 8(3), 38–45. doi:10.1207/s15327892mcp0803_7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, W. (2008). The meditational role of identification in the relationship between experience mode and self-efficacy: Enactive role-playing versus passive observation. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 11(6), 649–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, W., Lee, M., & Heeter, C. (2010). The effects of a serious game on role-taking and willingness to help. Journal of Communication, 60, 723–742. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010.01511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, S.-Y., Rangsipaht, S., & Thaipakdee, S. (2005). Measuring intercultural sensitivity: A comparative study of ethnic Chinese and Thai nationals. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 34(2), 119–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prathap, D. P., & Ponnusamy, K. A. (2009). A comparison of mass media channels in terms of knowledge retention. International Journal of Instructional Media, 36(1), 73–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramasubramanian, S. (2007). Media-based strategies to reduce racial stereotypes activated bv news stories. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 84(2), 249–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romano, D., & Brna, P. (2001). Presence and reflection in training: Support for learning to improve quality decision-making skills under time limitations. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 4(2), 265–277. doi:10.1089/109493101300117947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, R., Baughman, S., & Bailenson, J. (2013). Virtual superheroes: Using superpowers in virtual reality to encourage prosocial behavior. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e55003. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L., Kooper, R., Opdyke, D., Williford, J., & North, M. M. (1995). Effectiveness of virtual graded exposure in the treatment of acrophobia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 626–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savukynas, V. (2003). The other in society: Stereotypes and identity. Dialogue & Universalism, 13(1/2), 103–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, E. J. (1996). Muddles and huddles: Facilitating a multicultural workforce through team management theory. The Journal of Business Communication, 33(4), 459–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuemie, M., van der Straaten, P., Krijn, M., & van der Mast, C. (2001). Research on presence in virtual reality: A survey. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 4(2), 183–201. doi:10.1089/109493101300117884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidel, G. (1981). Cross-cultural training procedures: Their theoretical framework and evaluation. In S. Bochner (Ed.), The mediating person: Bridges between cultures (pp. 184–214). Cambridge, MA: G. K. Hall and Co. and Schenkman Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, M., Pertaub, D.-P., Barker, C., & Clark, D. M. (2006). An experimental study on fear of public speaking using a virtual environment. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 9(5), 627–633. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9.627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, W., & Stephan, C. (2013). Designing intercultural education and training programs: An evidence-based approach. International Journal on Intercultural Relations, 37(3), 277–286. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2012.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, A., Hernandez, J., Johnsen, K., Dickerson, R., Raij, A., Harrison, C., et al. (2006). The use of virtual patients to teach media student’s medical history taking and communication skills. American Journal of Surgery, 191(6), 806–811. doi:10.1016/j.amjsurg.2006.03.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stichter, J., Laffey, J., Galyen, K., & Herzog, M. (2014). iSocial: Delivering the social competence intervention for adolescents (SCI-A) in a 3D virtual learning environment for youth with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 417–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suied, C., Drettakis, G., Warusfel, O., & Viaud-Delmon, I. (2013). Auditory-visual virtual reality as a diagnostic and therapeutic tool for cynophobia. Cyberpsychology, Behavior & Social Networking, 16(2), 145–152. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.1568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamam, E. (2010). Examining Chen and Starosta’s model of intercultural sensitivity in a multiracial collectivistic country. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 39(3), 173–183. doi:10.1080/17475759.2010.534860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tüzün, H., & Özdinç, F. (2016). The effects of 3D multi-user virtual environments on freshman university students’ conceptual and spatial learning and presence in departmental orientation. Computers in Education, 94, 228–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vargas, J. A. (2006, February 14). Virtual reality prepares soldiers for war. The Washington Post. Retrieved from http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2006/02/13/AR2006021302437.html.

  • Wang, Y.-W., Davidson, M. M., Yashuko, O. F., Savoy, H. B., Tan, J. A., & Bleier, J. K. (2003). The scale of ethnocultural empathy: Development, validation, and reliability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50(2), 221–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.50.2.221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warburton, S. (2009). Second Life in higher educationAssessing the potential for and the barriers to deploying virtual worlds in learning and teaching. British Journal of Educational Technology, 40(3), 414–426. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8535.2009.00952.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, M. (2015, March 29). This company was 13 years early to virtual reality—And it’s getting ready to try again. Business Insider. Retrieved March 30, 2016, from http://www.businessinsider.com/second-life-is-still-around-and-getting-ready-to-conquer-virtual-reality-2015-3.

  • Zhong, Y., & Liu, C. (2014). A domain-oriented end-user design environment for generating interactive 3D virtual chemistry experiments. Multimedia Tools and Applications, 72(3), 2895–2924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would also like to acknowledge the assistance of Hyungwook Park, Ryan Tanay, Kevin (West) Bowers, Fang Fang Gao, and Evan Serge.

Funding

This study was funded by the Human Social Cultural and Behavioral Sciences (HSCB) Program of the U.S. Department of Defense (U.S. Army), contract #00066834.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amy Jo Coffey.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

Research involving human participants and/or animals

The authors affirm that human participants provided informed consent prior to participation in this study.

Appendix: Sample of cultural content presented in stimuli—recorded text from Second China business scenario

Appendix: Sample of cultural content presented in stimuli—recorded text from Second China business scenario

When attending a Chinese business meeting, one should arrive 5 min early. Guests are greeted by the host’s representative and shown to a meeting room. Gifts are common in business and particular attention should be paid to gift selection. Appropriate gifts include items which symbolize the giver’s culture like traditional art or books of photographs. Fine cognacs and pens are also good choices. Gifts to be avoided are clocks, umbrellas, white flowers, and green hats.

For the Chinese, landscape painting represented the universal longing to commune with nature. Chinese depictions of nature were seldom mere representations of the external world but also self-expression and criticism toward the society at the time. Daoism may have contributed to the rise of landscape painting due to its emphasis on how minor human presence is in the greater cosmos.

Seniority is very important in Chinese business contexts and is illustrated in a number of ways. In a Chinese meeting room, the principal guest and host usually sit at the middle of the table with the guest facing the door. Low-ranking guests sit toward the ends of the table. When giving out business cards or other business materials, one should start with the most senior people rather than with those sitting closest to you.

The most common refreshment served at a Chinese business meeting is tea. It is usually served boiling hot. In a formal business situation, there is no need to drink more than a couple of sips of tea. If you do drink more, it will be refilled periodically. If you have the opportunity to pour tea for someone, it will be interpreted as a gesture of respect. In a business environment, the pouring of tea is usually performed by a subordinate.

In modern settings, long tables in the center of meeting rooms may be decorated with flags of the visiting party’s country. In addition, one may encounter fresh fruit or bottled water on tables in meeting rooms. Fresh flowers can also be found in some modern conference rooms in China. White flowers are not usually found because white is a funeral color in Chinese culture. Flowers of bright colors like red are preferred.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Coffey, A.J., Kamhawi, R., Fishwick, P. et al. The efficacy of an immersive 3D virtual versus 2D web environment in intercultural sensitivity acquisition. Education Tech Research Dev 65, 455–479 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-017-9510-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-017-9510-9

Keywords

Navigation