Skip to main content
Log in

The influence of narrative and expository lesson text structures on knowledge structures: alternate measures of knowledge structure

  • Development Article
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This investigation applies two approaches for representing and comparing text structures as undirected network graphs to describe the influence of narrative and expository lesson texts on readers’ knowledge structure elicited as free recall. Narrative and expository lesson texts and undergraduate participants’ free recall essays (n = 90) from a study by Wolfe and Mienko (Br J Educ Psychol 77, 541–564, 2007) were reanalyzed for lexical proximity as sequential occurrence of selected important terms in the text and as actual minimum distances between these terms. The proximity data were then rendered as Pathfinder networks for analysis. Compared to human-rater benchmark measures, the convergent validity of the sequential approach (range of r = .53 to .83, median r = .70) was a little better than that of the minimum distance approach (.51 to .80, median r = .67). Further, we anticipated that the lesson text structure would be reflected in the text structure of the free recall essays, but this was not observed. On average, the essays in all three lesson conditions tended to converge on a sequential expository structure. Further, compared to the expository lesson texts, the narrative lesson text had a distinctly different influence on posttest recall essay text structures. Overall then, the sequential occurrence approach appears to provide a reasonably good, automatically derived method for representing and comparing lesson texts and participants’ essays as network graphs. If further confirmed and fully automated, there is a wide range of application of such measurement approaches for learning and research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbott, H. P. (2002). The Cambridge introduction to narrative. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlanga, A. J., van Rosmalen, P., Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Sloep, P. B. (2012). Exploring formative feedback on textual assignments with the help of automatically created visual representations. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 28, 146–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18, 255–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CCSS. (2010). Common core state standards for English language arts and literacy in history/social studies, science, and technical subjects; Appendix A: Research supporting key elements of the standards. Available from the Council of Chief State School Officers, Washington, DC. Retrieved July 22, 2014, from http://www.corestandards.org/assets/Appendix_A.pdf.

  • Clariana, R. B. (2004). Analysis of lexical aggregates (ALA-Reader). Retrieved July 22, 2014, from http://www.personal.psu.edu/rbc4/score.htm.

  • Clariana, R. B. (2010). Deriving group knowledge structure from semantic maps and from essays. In D. Ifenthaler, P. Pirnay-Dummer, & N. M. Seel (Eds.), Computer-based diagnostics and systematic analysis of knowledge (pp. 117–130). New York, NY: Springer.

  • Clariana, R. B., & Koul, R. (2004). A computer-based approach for translating text into concept map-like representations. In A. J. Canas, J. D. Novak, & F. M. Gonzales (Eds.), Concept maps: Theory, methodology, technology (Vol. 2). In Proceedings of the first international conference on concept mapping, Pamplona, Spain, Sep. 14–17, pp. 131–134.

  • Clariana, R. B., & Prestera, G. E. (2009). The effects of lesson screen background color on declarative and structural knowledge. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 40, 281–293.

  • Clariana, R. B., & Taricani, E. M. (2010). The consequences of increasing the number of terms used to score open-ended concept maps. International Journal of Instructional Media, 37, 163–173.

  • Clariana, R. B., & Wallace, P. E. (2007). A computer-based approach for deriving and measuring individual and team knowledge structure from essay questions. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 37, 209–225.

  • Clariana, R. B., Wallace, P. E., & Godshalk, V. M. (2009). Deriving and measuring group knowledge structure from essays: The effects of anaphoric reference. Educational Technology Research and Development, 57, 725–737.

  • Cunningham, L. J., & Gall, M. D. (1990). The effects of expository and narrative prose on student achievement and attitudes toward textbooks. Journal of Experimental Education, 58, 165–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Draper, D. (2010). The instructional effects of knowledge-based community of practice learning environments on student achievement and knowledge convergence. Doctoral dissertation retrieved July 22, 2014, from https://etda.libraries.psu.edu/paper/10629/.

  • Dubeck, L. W., Bruce, M. H., Schmuckler, J. S., Moshier, S. E., & Boss, J. E. (1990). Science fiction aids science teaching. The Physics Teacher, 28, 316–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, T. E., Johnson, P. J., & Acton, W. H. (1991). Assessing structural knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 88–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ifenthaler, D. (2010). Relational, structural, and semantic analysis of graphical representations and concept maps. Educational Technology Research and Development, 58, 81–97. doi:10.1007/s11423-008-9087-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Hernandez-Serrano, J. (2002). Case-based reasoning and instructional design: Using stories to support problem-solving. Educational Technology Research and Development, 50(2), 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, M. K. (2012). Cross-validation study of methods and technologies to assess mental models in a complex problem solving situation. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 703–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1977). On comprehending stories. In M. A. Just & P. A. Carpenter (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension (pp. 33–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klois, S. S., Segers, E., Clariana, R. B., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Effects of links in children’s digital text comprehension. A presentation with paper at the Twentieth Annual Meeting Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Hong Kong (July).

  • Koul, R., Clariana, R. B., & Salehi, R. (2005). Comparing several human and computer-based methods for scoring concept maps and essays. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 32, 261–273.

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning, legitimate peripheral participation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F., Brandt, D. M., & Bluth, G. J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key for reading comprehension of ninth-grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 72–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F., Young, C. J., & Bartlett, B. J. (1989). Memory improved: Reading and memory enhancement across the life span through strategic text structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, B., & Newton, E. (2002). An examination of the informational text genre in basal readers. Reading Psychology, 23, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roller, C. M., & Schreiner, R. (1985). The effects of narrative and expository organizational instruction on sixth-grade children’s comprehension of expository and narrative prose. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 6, 27–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sáenz, L. M., & Fuchs, L. S. (2002). Examining the reading difficulty of secondary students with learning disabilities: Expository versus narrative text. Remedial and Special Education, 23, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Körkel, J., & Weinert, F. E. (1989). Domain-specific knowledge and memory performance: A comparison of high- and low-aptitude children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 306–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schvaneveldt, R. W. (Ed.). (1990). Pathfinder associative networks: Studies in knowledge organization. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, E. M. (1995a). Narrative comprehension and the role of deictic shift theory. In J. F. Duchan, G. A. Bruder, & L. E. Hewitt (Eds.), Deixis in narrative: A cognitive science perspective (pp. 3–17). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, E. M. (1995b). A cognitive-phenomenological theory of fictional narrative. In J. F. Duchan, G. A. Bruder, & L. E. Hewitt (Eds.), Deixis in narrative: A cognitive science perspective (pp. 61–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spilich, G. J., Vesonder, G. T., Chiesi, H. L., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 275–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storey, D. C. (1982). Reading in the content areas: Fictionalized biographies and diaries for social studies. The Reading Teacher, 35, 796–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, M. B. (2005). Memory for narrative and expository text: Independent influences of semantic associations and text organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 359–364.

  • Wolfe, M. B., & Mienko, J. A. (2007). Learning and memory of factual content from narrative and expository text. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 541–564.

  • Zabrucky, K. M., & Moore, D. (1999). Influence of text genre on adults’ monitoring of understanding and recall. Educational Gerontology, 25, 691–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roy B. Clariana.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Clariana, R.B., Wolfe, M.B. & Kim, K. The influence of narrative and expository lesson text structures on knowledge structures: alternate measures of knowledge structure. Education Tech Research Dev 62, 601–616 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-014-9348-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-014-9348-3

Keywords

Navigation