Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

A multi-dimensional model of the origins of attitude certainty: teachers’ attitudes toward attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

  • Published:
Social Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research tested and extended a laboratory-derived model of the origins of attitude certainty using a real attitude object: Teaching children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In Study 1, an experiment manipulated the amount of information, thought, and consistency of information delivered to pre-service teachers (\(n = 224\)) with no prior experience with ADHD. Structural equation modelling (SEM) of Study 1 did not support the original model, in which relationships between attitude certainty and objective knowledge, thought, and consistency were mediated by perceived knowledge, thought, and ambivalence. Instead, objective amount of information, thought, and consistency interacted in their effect on attitude certainty. Study 2 (\(n = 368\)) used a survey to test whether experiences with ADHD (personal, direct and indirect) among in-service and pre-service teachers were antecedents of attitude certainty, and to test perceived accessibility as a mediator. SEM supported both these hypotheses. Perceived accessibility and perceived knowledge mediated the relationship between attitude certainty and prior experiences with ADHD, and between attitude certainty and objective knowledge. Together, the results suggest that psychological processes underlying strong attitude certainty differ according to the familiarity and personal relevance of the attitude object, and the context and stage of attitude formation. The results have practical utility for teacher training at pre-service and in-service levels.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Details of the statements and pre-testing results can be obtained by contacting the first author.

  2. Interested readers may contact the first author for copies of the questionnaire and information sets.

  3. Descriptive statistics and correlations among attitude strength variables may be obtained from the first author.

  4. Interested readers may contact the first author for copies of the questionnaires.

  5. Descriptive statistics and correlations between all variables for Model 1 and Model 2 may be obtained from the first author.

References

  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2000). Attitudes and the attitude-behaviour relation: Reasoned and automatic processes. European Review of Social Psychology, 11(1), 1–33. doi:10.1080/14792779943000116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. L., Watt, S. E., Noble, W., & Shanley, D. C. (2012). Knowledge of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and attitudes toward teaching children with ADHD: The role of teaching experience. Psychology in the Schools, 49(6), 511–525. doi:10.1002/pits.21617.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barbaresi, W. J., & Olsen, R. D. (1998). An ADHD educational intervention for elementary school teachers: A pilot study. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 19(2), 94–100. doi:10.1097/00004703-199804000-00005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barden, J., & Petty, R. E. (2008). The mere perception of elaboration creates attitude certainty: Exploring the thoughtfulness heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 489–509. doi:10.1037/a0012559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgemental versus operative indexes of psychological attributes: The case of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(4), 637–653. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bizer, G. Y., Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Memory-based versus on-line processing: Implications for attitude strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 646–653. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modelling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications and programming. Mahawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., Pomerantz, E. M., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (1995). Structural consistency and attitude strength. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedants and consequences (pp. 387–412). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of word sin episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Belmont, CA: Thomson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1998). Attitude structure and function. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 269–322). Boston: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaton, A. A., Majka, E. A., & Visser, P. S. (2008). Emerging perspectives on the structure and function of attitude strength. European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 165–201. doi:10.1080/10463280802383791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esses, V. M., & Maio, G. R. (2002). Expanding the assessment of attitude components and structure: The benefits of open-ended measures. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 71–101). London: Wiley Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 247–282). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Attitudinal qualities relating to the strength of the attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 398–408. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(78)90035-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1981). Direct experience and attitude-behavior consistency. Advances in experimental social psychology, 14, 161–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, S. R., Holtz, R., & Miller, N. (1995). Attitude certainty. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 215–245). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haddock, G., Rothman, A. J., Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Forming judgements of attitude certainty, intensity and importance: The role of subjective experiences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(7), 771–782. doi:10.1177/0146167299025007001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1998). On the use of open-ended measures to assess attitudinal components. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 129–149. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1998.tb01161.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1999). Cognition, affect, and the prediction of social attitudes. European Review of Social Psychology, 10(1), 75–99. doi:10.1080/14792779943000026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Peterson, C. K., & Harris, C. R. (2009). Jealousy: Novel methods and neural correlates. Emotion, 9(1), 113–117. doi:10.1037/a0014117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. (2004). Experimental design and causality in social psychology. In C. Sansone, C. C. Morf, & A. T. Panter (Eds.), The Sage handbook of methods in social psychology (pp. 237–264). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millenium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420. doi:10.1080/03637750903310360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holbrook, A. L., Berent, M. K., Krosnick, J. A., Visser, P. S., & Boninger, D. S. (2005). Attitude importance and the accumulation of attitude-relevant knowledge in memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 749–769. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, R. W., Verplanken, B., & van Kippenberg, A. (2003). From repetition to conviction: Attitude accessibility as a determinant of attitude certainty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 594–601. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00038-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jerome, L., Gordon, M., & Hustler, P. (1994). A comparison of American and Canadian teachers’ knowledge and attitudes towards attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 39(9), 563–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, H. A., & Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2008). Efficacy of teacher in-service training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychology in the Schools, 45(10), 918–929. doi:10.1002/pits.20342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kos, J. M., & Richdale, A. L. (2004). The history of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Australian Journal of Learning Disabilities, 9(1), 22–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kos, J. M., Richdale, A. L., & Jackson, M. S. (2004). Knowledge about attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparison of in-service and pre-service teachers. Psychology in the Schools, 41(5), 517–526. doi:10.1002/pits.10178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Attitude strength: An overview. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (Vol. 1, pp. 1–24). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ledgerwood, A., & Callahan, S. P. (2012). The social side of abstraction: Psychological distance enhances conformity to group norms. Psychological Science,. doi:10.1177/0956797611435920.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 130–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maio, G. R., Olson, J. M., Bernard, M. M., & Luke, M. A. (2003). Ideologies, values, attitudes and behavior. In J. Delamater (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 283–308). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, G. (2012). Revisting truth or triviality: The external validity of research in the psychological laboratory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(2), 109–117. doi:10.1177/1745691611432343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • New South Wales Department of Education. (1993). Attention deficit disorder: Support statement for school personnel. Sydney: NSW Department of School Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrocelli, J. V., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Unpacking attitude certainty: Attitude clarity and attitude correctness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 30–41. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Haugtvedt, C. P., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Elaboration as a determinant of attitude strength: Creating attitudes that are persistent, resistant, and predictive of behavior. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 93–130). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(6), 942–948. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.6.942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. doi:10.3758/BF03206553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raden, D. (1985). Strength-related attitude dimensions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 312–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rucker, D. D., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2008). What’s in a frame anyway? A meta-cognitive analysis of the impact of one versus two sided message framing on attitude certainty? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18, 137–149. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2008.01.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmelainen, P. (2002). Trends in the prescribing of stimulant medication for the treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in children and adolescents in NSW (Vol. 13, pp. 1–65). Canberra: NSW Department of Health.

  • Sciutto, M. J., & Terjesen, M. D. (2000). Teachers’ knowledge and misperceptions of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychology in the Schools, 37(2), 115–122. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6807(200003)37:2<115:AID-PITS3>3.0.CO;2-5.

  • Scott, W. A. (1968). Attitude measurement. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 204–273). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influence of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., Fabrigar, L. R., Macdougall, B. L., & Weisenthal, N. L. (2008). The role of amount, cognitive elaboration, and structural consistency of attitude-relevant knowledge in the formation of attitude certainty. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 280–295. doi:10.1002/ejsp.447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, M. M., Zanna, M. P., & Griffin, D. W. (1995). Let’s not be indifferent about (attitudinal) ambivalence. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 361–386). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomsen, C. J., Borgida, E., & Lavine, H. (1995). The causes and consequences of personal involvement. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 191–214). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tormala, Z. L., Clarkson, J. J., & Henderson, M. D. (2011). Does fast or slow evaluation foster greater certainty? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(3), 422–434. doi:10.1177/01461672103977378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Attitude certainty: A review of past findings and emerging perspectives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 1(1), 469–492. doi:10.1111/j.1751-90004.2007.00025.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vereb, R. L., & DiPerna, J. C. (2004). Teachers’ knowledge of ADHD, treatments for ADHD, and treatment acceptability: An initial investigation. School Psychology Review, 33(3), 421–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Visser, P. S., Bizer, G. Y., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.). (2006). Exploring the latent structure of strength-related attitude attributes (Vol. 38). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Visser, P. S., & Mirabile, R. R. (2004). Attitudes in the social context: The impact of social network composition on individual-level attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 779–795. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wan, E. W., Rucker, D. D., Tormala, Z. L., & Clarkson, J. J. (2010). The effect of reguatory depletion on attitude certainty. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 531–541. doi:10.1509/jmkr.47.3.531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D. T., Downing, J., Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Measures and manipulations of strength-related properties of attitudes: Current practice and future directions. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 455–488). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, J., Taylor, M., Houghton, S., & Hudyma, S. (2005). A comparison of teachers’ and parents’ knowledge and beliefs about attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). School Psychology International, 26(2), 192–208. doi:10.1177/0143034305052913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilens, T. E., Adler, L. A., Adams, J., Sgambati, S., Rotrosen, J., Sawtelle, R., et al. (2008). Misuse and diversion of stimulants prescribed for ADHD: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(1), 21–31. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a56f1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(131–134), doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00355.x.

  • Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Biek, M. (1995). Working knowledge and attitude strength: An information processing analysis. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 283–314). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank everyone who helped recruit participants and all the teachers and education students who participated. Thank you to Dr. Tony Marks for his valued statistical consultation, Emeritus Professor Bettina Cass for her supportive mentoring, and to Emma Peart for sharing her experiences in the classroom that formed the basis of the vignette.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donnah L. Anderson.

Appendix: Assumptions

Appendix: Assumptions

1.1 Study 1

Following MacCallum et al. (1996) power tables for 72 degrees of freedom, \(N = 168\) was needed for power of .80 and a close fit. Close fit refers to testing the null hypothesis that the model fits the population at \(p<.05\) (MacCallum et al. 1996). The achieved sample size of 224 pre-service teachers was satisfactory. Multivariate normality, multicollinearity, linearity, homoscedasticity and multivariate outlier assumptions were met. Due to concerns about significant positive skewness at \(p< .001\) for five variables, PK2 (\(z_\mathrm{skew} =3.65\)), C1 (\(z_\mathrm{skew} = 3.44\)), C2 (\(z_\mathrm{skew} = 3.42\)), PT1 (\(z_\mathrm{skew} = 3.78\)), and PT3 (\(z_\mathrm{skew} = 4.06\)), bias-corrected bootstrapped standard errors were used in the structural equation analyses (Byrne 2001). The bootstrapped procedure is free of assumptions of normality and provides more accurate estimates of parameter values than parametric methods when there are non-normal variables (Byrne 2001). All variables were left untransformed to maintain interpretability and to allow comparison to Smith et al.’s (2008) results.

1.2 Study 2

1.2.1 Missing data: combined sample

Data were collected from 327 pre-service teachers and 127 in-service teachers. Contrasts of missing cases (five cases on total knowledge, 63 cases on objective elaboration and objective ambivalence) versus complete cases on all the perceived attitude strength variables and total knowledge were non-significant (\(p> .001\)). Thus, data was Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) (Byrne 2001; Kline 2005). Considering the large sample size and MCAR evidence, the 63 cases with missing data, which included 5 cases with missing scores on total knowledge, were excluded from the analysis, leaving complete cases for 111 in-service teachers and 280 pre-service teachers.

1.2.2 Model 1 assumptions

Assumptions for univariate outliers, homoscedasticity and linearity were met. Exclusion of 11 multivariate outliers using Mahalanobis distances for 15 predictors, \(p< .001\) (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007), left a sample of 269 pre-service teachers, which had good statistical power (MacCallum et al. 1996). Due to concerns about normality (Mardia criterion for multivariate kurtosis, \(z = 5.98\), PAM3, \(z_\mathrm{skew}= - 8.61\)) bias corrected bootstrapped standard errors were used in the analyses (Byrne 2001). The variables were left untransformed to aid interpretability. A multicollinearity issue was detected. The items PK2 and PK3 were strongly correlated, \(r (267) = .94, p< .001\), and loaded onto the smallest eigenvalue at .73 and .76, respectively. This was resolved by using the average of the two variables (abbreviated PK2a, see Table 3) in the SEM analyses (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). The final Durbin-Watson statistic was 2.06, which supported independence of errors among the endogenous variables.

1.2.3 Model 2 assumptions

Exclusion of 14 multivariate outliers detected using Mahalanobis distances for 19 predictors at \(p< .001\), left a sample size of 366 (267 education students and 99 in-service teachers). Linearity assumptions were met, however heteroscedasticity was detected. Due to concerns about normality (direct experience, \(z_{skew }= 17.92, z_{kurtosis} = 23.19\); personal experience, \(z_\mathrm{skew} = 6.98\); indirect experience, \(z_\mathrm{skew} = 5.10\); Mardia criterion for multivariate kurtosis, \(z = 8.82)\), bootstrapped bias corrected standard errors were used (Byrne 2001). The variables were left untransformed to aid interpretability. The items PK2 and PK3 were strongly correlated, \(r (364) = .93, p< .001\), and loaded onto the smallest eigenvalue at .77 and .75, respectively. The average of these two variables was used in the SEM (PK2a).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Anderson, D.L., Watt, S.E. & Shanley, D.C. A multi-dimensional model of the origins of attitude certainty: teachers’ attitudes toward attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Soc Psychol Educ 17, 19–50 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-013-9235-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-013-9235-5

Keywords

Navigation