Skip to main content
Log in

Evoking Equanimity: Silent Interaction Rituals in Vipassana Meditation Retreats

  • Published:
Qualitative Sociology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Studies on the benefits of meditation show that the practice reduces stress and helps achieve a feeling of equanimity and peacefulness. These studies have tended to view meditation as an individual endeavor; however, many people learn to practice meditation in a group. Drawing on literature that connects the self, emotional experience and social interaction, I suggest that meditation-based equanimity is not only a psychological state but also a social attitude that is cultivated and learned in a unique silent interaction order. Based on participant observation and in-depth interviews with participants in vipassana meditation retreats in Israel and the United States, I analyze instances of silent social interaction that take place in meditation centers. The analysis reveals a sociological understanding of equanimity as a sociality of “non-engagement” which serves as a grey zone between full engagement and complete disengagement. As I show, participants in meditation retreats go through a gradual process of learning how to be with others while not directly attending to them. This form of being together allows for the emergence of silent social attunement that facilitates equanimity. Participants purposely cultivate and perform equanimity with and for others, but eventually it takes over the self, leading to an experience of self-transformation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Recent research on religious transformation began to put more emphasis on the emotional sides of religious experience. See, for example, Lee et al. 2013.

  2. This paper focuses on relatively non-experienced practitioners and their first encounters with meditative equanimity. These first encounters (i.e. the first meditation retreat) are usually described by practitioners as most significant in their experience of self-change, even when recalling the experience after a few years. Advanced vipassana practitioners do make use of Buddhist concepts and ideas (see Pagis 2010b).

  3. See Pagis 2009 for the claim that self-reflexivity is not necessary mediated by language.

  4. Theravada Buddhism is commonly practiced in Southeast Asia. The popularization of vipassana to the laity began in Burma around 1950, moved to other parts of South-East Asia (such as Thailand and Cambodia) and to non-Buddhist parts of the world (including India).

  5. This emphasis on mental health led to programs that are delivered in hospitals, such as Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR), that is based on a mix of Zen and vipassana. MBSR is taught in weekly collective group meetings; however, John Kabat Zinn, the founder of the program, recommends attending silent meditation retreats and reports that he himself participates regularly in such retreats.

  6. The body, sensations, mind and objects of the mind are the four objects of observation in the Satipatthana Sutta, the Theravada Buddhist text which includes instructions for the practice of vipassana meditation.

  7. In South-East Asia two main lines of teachers opened lay vipassana centers. The most popular meditation centers today in Burma and Thailand follow the teachings of Mahasi Sayadaw. Meditation centers that follow the teachings of S.N. Goenka have become extremely popular in India. Though the lineages of both Mahasi Sayadaw and S.N. Goenka track back to the same teacher (Ledi Sayadow), they teach a slightly different version of vipassana. The vipassana practice introduced in this study follows S.N. Goenka who places a specific emphasis on being mindful of bodily sensations and includes a method of bodily sweeping. In the non-Buddhist world we can find meditation centers that either adhere to one version of vipassana or teach a combination of both.

  8. Even though Israel is located in the Middle East, it is highly influenced by Anglo-American culture, especially among secular, educated young adults. See Ram 2007.

  9. Not everyone that takes a meditation course experiences this self-change. Those who do not experience a change will rarely continue to meditate. While I have spoken with people who did not find the course satisfactory, this paper concentrates on people that did experience a positive effect and regard meditation as an important part of their lives.

  10. Studies on silence in interaction reveal that in locations influenced by Anglo-American culture prolonged moments of silence are considered negative and are experienced as a discomfort pose in the flow of interaction (Bilmes 1994; Gurevitch 1989; Jaworski 1993). However, there are places, such as Finland, in which silence is regarded as a natural part of interaction (Basso 1970; Lehtonen and Sajavaara 1985).

  11. These conditions are not limited to Buddhist retreats. For an analysis of the process of attunement into silence in the context of Christian meditation retreats see Mermis Cava (2007).

  12. The recent finding of mirror neurons in the brains of primates and humans can explain the tendency to imitate the bodily movements of others, which can lead to both bodily and emotional synchronization (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia 2008).

  13. For a discussion of other worlds of silence see Kurzon 2007.

References

  • Basso, Keith. 1970. To give up on words: Silence in western Apache culture. Southern Journal of Anthropology 26: 213–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilmes, Jack. 1994. Constituting silence: Life in the world of total meaning. Semiotica 98: 73–87.

  • Brown, Kirk Warren, and Richard M. Ryan. 2003. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84: 822–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cadge, Wendy. 2005. Heartwood: The first generation of Theravada Buddhism in America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, Randall. 1989. Toward a neo-Meadian sociology of mind. Symbolic Interaction 12: 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, Randall. 2004. Interaction ritual chains. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley, Charles. H. 1998/1902. On self and social organization. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • Gecas, Victor, and J. Peter Burke. 1995. Self and identity. In Sociological Perspectives on Social Psychology, ed. S. Karen Cook, Fine Gary Alan, and S. James House, 41–67. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, Erving. 1963. The interaction order. American Sociological Review 48: 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, Erving. 1967. Interaction ritual: Essays on face -to-face behavior. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, Erving. 1981. Forms of talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gombrich, Richard. 1983. From monastery to meditation center: Lay meditation in contemporary Sri Lanka. In Buddhist studies ancient and modern, ed. Philip Denwood and Alexander Piatigorsky, 20–34. London: Curzon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurevitch, Zalli. 1989. Distance and conversation. Symbolic Interaction 12: 251–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, Elaine, John Cacioppo, and Richard L. Rapson. 1994. Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, William. 1981/1890. The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.

  • Jaworski, Adam. 1993. The power of silence: Social and pragmatic perspectives. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jordt, Ingrid. 2007. Burma’s mass lay meditation movement: Buddhism and the cultural construction of power. Athens: Ohio University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, Jon. 2005. Coming to our senses: Healing ourselves and the world through mindfulness. New York: Hyperion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, Jack. 1999. How emotions work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, Kurt. 1935. Principles of Gestalt psychology. London: Lund Humphries.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurzon, Dennis. 2007. Towards a typology of silence. Journal of Pragmatics 39: 1673–1688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, Matthew T., Margaret Poloma, and Stephen G. Post. 2013. The heart of religion: Spiritual empowerment, benevolence, and the experience of God’s love. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehtonen, Jaakko, and Kari Sajavaara. 1985. The silent Finn. In Perspectives on silence, ed. Deborah Tannen and Muriel Saville-Troike, 193–201. Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, Antoine, Julie Brefczynski-Lewis, Tom Johnstone, and Richard Davidson. 2008. Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3(3): e1897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead George, H. 1934. Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mermis-Cava, Jonathan. 2007. Relating with silence: Christian meditation and the production of roles, relationships, and culture. Unpublished Dissertation. Davis: University of California, Davis.

  • Pagis, Michal. 2009. Embodied self-reflexivity. Social Psychology Quarterly 72: 265–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagis, Michal. 2010a. Producing intersubjectivity in silence: An ethnography of meditation practices. Ethnography 11: 309–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagis, Michal. 2010b. From abstract concepts to experiential knowledge: Embodying enlightenment in a meditation center. Qualitative Sociology 33: 469–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagis, Michal. 2013. Religious self-constitution: A relational perspective. In Religion on the Edge, ed. Courtney Bender, Wendy Cadge, Peggy Levitt, and David Smilde, 92–114. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perbish, Charles S., and Martin Baumann. 2002. Westward Dharma: Buddhism beyond Asia. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ram, Uri. 2007. The globalization of Israel: McWorld in Tel Aviv, Jihad in Jerusalem. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, Giacomo, and Corrado Sinigaglia. 2008. Mirrors in the brain. How we share our actions and emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saville-Troike, Muriel. 1985. The place of silence in an integrated theory of communication. In Perspectives on silence, ed. Tannen Deborah and Saville-Troike Muriel. Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheff, Thomas. 1997. Emotions, the social bond and human reality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, Christian. 2003. Moral believing animals: Human personhood and culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Snow David, A., and Machalek Richard. 1984. The Sociology of conversion. Annual Review of Sociology 10: 167–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stefan, Timmermans, and Iddo Tavory. 2012. Theory construction in qualitative research: From grounded theory to abductive analysis. Sociological Theory 30: 167–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern Daniel, N. 1985. The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, Fritz, Leonard Martin, and Sabine Stepper. 1988. Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 768–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Summers-Effler, Erika. 2002. The micro-potential for social change: Emotion, consciousness and social movement formation. Sociological Theory 20: 41–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weigert, Andrew J., and Victor Gecas. 2003. Self. In Handbook of symbolic interactionism, ed. Larry T. Reynolds and Nancy J. Herman-Kinney, 267–88. Walnut Creek: Alta Mira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winchester, Daniel. 2008. Embodying the faith: Religious practice and the making of a Muslim moral habitus. Social Forces 86: 1753–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zerubavel, Eviatar. 2006. The elephant in the room: Silence and denial in everyday life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I wish to thank the many people who opened their hearts and shared their experiences with me. This work could not have been done without their insights and wisdom. The analysis and conclusions are my own. I would like to thank Jonathan Mermis-Cava whose work on silent interaction in Christian meditation retreats inspired this paper. I am grateful to Erika Summers-Effler, Iddo Tavory and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this article. A previous version of this paper was presented at the 2011 American Sociological Association Annual Meeting in Las Vegas.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michal Pagis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pagis, M. Evoking Equanimity: Silent Interaction Rituals in Vipassana Meditation Retreats. Qual Sociol 38, 39–56 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11133-014-9295-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11133-014-9295-7

Keywords

Navigation