Skip to main content
Log in

Is monitoring one’s actions causally relevant to choking under pressure?

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. After graduating from high school at age 15 and before entering college, I spent a number of years working as a professional ballet dancer with North Carolina Dance Theatre (now Charlotte Ballet), among other companies.

  2. A choke, as I understand it is performing not necessarily far, far worse than ever before (I’m a pushover for manipulated literary allusions, whether perfectly accurate or not) but far, far worse than would be expected. Now, do cases that we refer to as chokes actually require explanation or are they merely cases of poor performance that would be predicted statically? Psychologists who aim to explain choking in terms of increased attention to movement assume that it is a phenomenon that requires explanation; I shall do the same.

  3. However, see Toner, Moran and Montero (forthcoming).

  4. I would like to thank Lorenzo Ruffo, an undergraduate student at the College of Staten Island, for drawing my attention to this issue.

  5. Also, high anxiety induces various physiological changes that appear to hinder performance. The fight-or-flight response which anxiety produces shunts blood flow to the larger muscles, leaving cold feet and hands, and thus motor skills relying on the hands or feet may be harmed. It can cause loss of peripheral vision, increased perspiration, and tremors. These points may be obvious, but they are often not mentioned in the literature on choking. The choke is thought to be something different in kind. But is it?

  6. See also Ravn and Christensen (2013).

  7. Might their performance be affected by the fact that they knew that after playing, they would need to record what was going on in their minds? It certainly might, however, although this might be relevant to the question of how much athletes think during games which they are not asked to do this, I don’t think it is relevant to the question of whether thinking interferes with performing. Even if the athletes end up thinking more than they normally would, it would seem that their reports of the effectiveness of their thoughts would not be significantly affected.

  8. This is not to say that simply thinking about stereotypes helps avoid the detrimental effects of stereotyped discourse, for as indicated by Beilock (2011, pp. 165–166, and 201), in reference primarily to a study on racial stereotype threat, (Steele and Aronson 1995), it seems that merely mentioning a stereotype can hinder performance on standardized tests (because, she hypothesizes, the subjects will have this stereotype in mind while taking the tests.) However, I take it that the situation I am describing is relevantly different primarily because the person who might make stereotypical remarks about girls and boys is not the one who is overtly negatively affected by the remarks; rather, it is the little girls who are always complimented on their looks are (or at least is seems reasonable to think that at least in our culture this could have a negative effect.) Another difference is that what I think is useful to keep in mind is not just the stereotype, or not even just the idea that there such a view is mistaken, but the idea that making remarks in accord with the stereotype should be avoided, the idea that you should not complement a girl’s looks. (It might be interesting to test how girl’s performance on standardized tests is affected by priming the idea that although there is a stereotype that they do worse than boy’s, there is no truth to it and that in taking the test they should overcome this and prove that they are just as (if not more) capable than boys.) Still another differences is that presumably making a casual remark to a girl takes far fewer cognitive resources than taking a standardized test and so diverting cognitive resources to the thought that one needs to say this rather than that would presumably not have a negative effect on one’s ability to make such claims whereas diverting cognitive resources even to thoughts about avoiding stereotypes might have a detrimental effect on the highly demanding task of taking a test. For other ways in which focusing more and thinking about one’s ordinary actions as one is performing them can improve those actions, see

References

  • Ashcraft, M., & Kirk, E. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. E. (1984). Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. (2011). Choke: What the secrets of the brain reveal about getting it right when you have to. New York: Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S., & Carr, T. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S., & DeCaro, M. (2007). From poor performance to success under stress: working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 983–998.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S., Carr, T., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(1), 6–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S., Bertenthal, B., McCoy, A., & Carr, T. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 373–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S., Bertenthal, B., Hoerger, M., & Carr, T. (2007). When does haste make waste? Speed-accuracy tradeoff, skill level, and the tools of the trade. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(4), 340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, D., Jones, B., Fairweather, M., Doolan, S., & Priestly, N. (2001). Examining anxiety associated changes in movement patterns. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 31, 223–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darke, S. (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 145–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCaro, M., Thomas, R., Albert, N., & Beilock. (2011). Choking under pressure multiple routes to skill failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1998). Working memory capacity in high trait-anxious and repressor groups. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 697–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1910/1997). How we think. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications.

  • Dreyfus (2013). The myth of the pervasiveness of the mental. In J. Schear ed., Mind, reason, and being-in-the-world: The McDowell-Dreyfus Debate, Routledge.

  • Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1986). Mind over Machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the age of the computer. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., & Kelly, S. (2011). Saving the sacred from the axial revolution. Inquiry, 54(2), 195–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, A. K. (2008). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on development of superior expert performance. In The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance, edited by K.A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P.J. Feltovich and R.R. Hoffman. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 683–704.

  • Ericsson, A. K. (2008b). Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: a general overview. Academic Emergency Medicine, 15, 988–994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Payne, S., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Trait anxiety, visuo-spatial processing, and working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1214–1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M. (1964). Perceptual-motor skill learning. In A. W. Melton (Ed.), Categories of human learning (pp. 243–285). NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont: Brooks Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, & Moskowitz. (2004). Coping Pitfalls and promises. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 745–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, P., Hodges, N. J., & Williams, A. M. (2005). Online attentional-focus manipulations in a soccer-dribbling task: implications for the proceduralization of motor skills. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 386–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freudenheim, A., Wulf, G., Madureira, F., Pasetto, S., & Correa, U. (2010). An external focus of attention results in greater swimming speed. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 5, 533–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S., Hirsch, C., & Mathews, A. (2008). Restriction of working memory capacity during worry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 712–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1927/1962). Being and Time, translated by J. Macquarrie and E. Robinson. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

  • Leavitt. (1979). Cognitive demands of skating and stickhandling in ice hockey. Canadian Journal of Applied Sports Science, 4(1), 46–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Donnellan, A. M. (1993). Individual differences in anxiety and the restriction of working memory capacity. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(2), 163–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W. (1992). Knowledge, (k)nerves and know-how: the role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. The British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W., Polman, R. C. J., & Hammond, N. V. (1993). Reinvestment: a dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 655–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1994/2002). The Phenomenology of Perception, trans. C. Smith, Routledge.

  • Montero, B. (2010). Does bodily awareness interfere with highly skilled movement? Inquiry, 53, 105–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montero, B. (2013). A dancer reflects: deliberation in action. In Shear (ed), Mind, reason and being-in-the-world: The McDowell-Dreyfus Debate. Routledge.

  • Montero, B. (forthcoming). The Myth of ‘Just do it’: Thought and Effort in Expert Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Montero, B., & Evans, C. (2011). Intuitions without concepts lose the game: mindedness in the art of chess. Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, 10(2), 175–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, A. R., Holt, N. L., Polman, R. C. J., & James, D. W. G. (2005). Stress and coping among international adolescent golfers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 330–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, A., Holt, N., Polman, R., & Bloomfield, J. (2006). Stressors, coping, and coping effectiveness among professional rugby union players. The Sport Psychologist, 20(3), 314–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R., Kuijpers, W., Kooijman, C. C., & Bakker, F. C. (2011). Thoughts and attention of athletes under pressure: skill-focus or performance worries? Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 24(1), 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravn, S., & Christensen, M. K. (2013). Listening to the body? How phenomenological insights can be used to explore a golfer’s experiences of the physicality of her body, Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health.

  • Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African-American. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoate, I., & Wulf, G. (2011). Does the attentional focus of swimmers affect their performance? International Journal of Sport Science & Coaching, 6, 99–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toner, J., Moran, A. and B. Montero (in progress). Considering the role of ‘time permitting’ deliberate behaviour in expert performance.

  • Toner, J., & Moran, A. (2011). The effects of conscious processing on golf putting proficiency and kinematics. Journal of Sports Sciences, 29(7), 673–683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toner, J. Jackson, F., Montero, B. and Moran, A. (in preparation a). Is conscious processing detrimental to sporting performance?

  • Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G. (2007). Attention and motor skill learning. Champaign: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 648–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G., & Su, J. (2007). An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in beginners and experts. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(4), 384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G., Shea, C., & Park, J. H. (2001). Attention and motor performance: preferences for and advantages of an external focus. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72(4), 335–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zollo, P. (2003). Interviews with Songwriters, De Capo Press.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barbara Gail Montero.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Montero, B.G. Is monitoring one’s actions causally relevant to choking under pressure?. Phenom Cogn Sci 14, 379–395 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9400-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9400-0

Keywords

Navigation