Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

A View on Human Goal-Directed Activity and the Construction of Artificial Intelligence

  • Published:
Minds and Machines Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although activity aimed at the construction of artificial intelligence started about 60 years ago however, contemporary intelligent systems are effective in very narrow domains only. One of the reasons for this situation appears to be serious problems in the theory of intelligence. Intelligence is a characteristic of goal-directed systems and two classes of goal-directed systems can be derived from observations on animals and humans, one class is systems with innately and jointly determined goals and means. The other class contains systems that are able to construct arbitrary goals and means. It is suggested that the classes (that implicitly underlie most models of artificial intelligence) are insufficient to explain human goal-directed activity. A broader approach to goal-directed systems is considered. This approach suggests that humans are goal-directed systems that jointly synthesize arbitrary goals and means. Neural and psychological data favoring this hypothesis and its experimental validation are considered. A simple computer model based on the idea of joint synthesis to simulate goal-directed activity is presented. The usage of the idea of joint synthesis for the construction of artificial intelligence is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asaad, W. F., Rainer, G., & Miller, E. K. (2000). Task-specific neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 451–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bäck, T. (1996). Evolutionary algorithms in theory and practice: Evolution strategies, evolutionary programming, genetic algorithms. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Batuev, A. S. (1981). High integrative systems of the brain. Moscow: Nauka. (in Russian).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertino, E., Piero, G., & Zarria, B. C. (2001). Intelligent database systems. Reading: Addison-Wesley Professional.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buller, D. J. (1998) DeFreuding evolutionary psychology: Adaptation and human motivation. In Hardcastle, V. G. (Ed.), Psychology meets biology: Connections, conjectures, constraints. MIT Press/Bradford Books. http://cogprints.soton.ac.uk/documents/disk0/00/00/03/26/cog00000326-00/defreud.htm.

  • Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (1995). Soar and the case for unified theories of cognition. Cognition, 55(2), 115–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corkill, D. D. (1991). Blackboard systems. AI Expert, 6(9), 40–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corsini, R. J., & Auerbach, A. J. (1998). Concise encyclopedia of psychology. Wiley.

  • Craig, I. (1995). Blackboard systems (Tutorial monographs in artificial intelligence series). Ablex Publishing Corporation.

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 454–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends Neuroscience, 20, 451–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haykin, S. (1998). Neural networks: A comprehensive foundation. Prentice Hall, Englewood cliffs.

  • Heckhausen, H. (1980). Motivation und Handeln. Verlag: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. H. (1975). Adaptation in natural and artificial systems. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, P. (1998). Introduction to expert systems (3rd ed.). Addison Wesley.

  • Joseph, R. (1999). Frontal lobe psychopathology: Mania, depression, confabulation, catatonia, perseveration, obsessive compulsions, and schizophrenia. Psychiatry, 62(2), 138–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, K. P., Littman, M. L., & Moore, A. W. (1996). Reinforcement learning: A survey. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research., 4, 237–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. Andover, Hants: Tavistock. Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, B. T., & D’Esposito, M. (2005). Searching for “the top” in top-down control. Neuron, 48, 535–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodina, O. N., & Prudkov, P. N. (2005). The principle of joint synthesis in purposeful processes. MGU Bulletin Psychology, 2, 77–86. (in Russian).

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, S. J., & Norvig, P. (2003). Artificial intelligence: A modern approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A.G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Barrett, H. C. (2005). Resolving the debate on innate ideas: Learnability constraints and the evolved interpenetration of motivational and conceptual functions. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind: Structure and content. NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Velde, F., & de Kamps, M. (2003). A model of visual working memory in PFC. Neurocomputing, 52–54, 419–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Velde, F., & de Kamps, M. (2006). Neural blackboard architectures of combinatorial structures in cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 37–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory: Foundation, development, applications. New York: Brazillier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. N., & Grafman, J. (2003). Human prefrontal cortex: Processing and representational perspectives. Nature Review Neuroscience, 4, 139–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pavel N. Prudkov.

Appendix

Appendix

p1 = 1; p4 = 2; p5 = 0.3; p6 = 0.98; p7 = 0.5; p8 = 0.3; p9 = 0.8; p10 = 0.8, noise = a random number from 0 to 1.1.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Prudkov, P.N. A View on Human Goal-Directed Activity and the Construction of Artificial Intelligence. Minds & Machines 20, 363–383 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-010-9218-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-010-9218-7

Keywords

Navigation