Skip to main content
Log in

The Dud Effect: Adding Highly Dissimilar Fillers Increases Confidence in Lineup Identifications

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

Recent research in decision-making has demonstrated the “dud-alternative effect”—the tendency to become more confident that a chosen response option is correct if it is surrounded by implausible response options (Windschitl & Chambers, J Exp Psychol 30:198–215, 2004). This finding may be applicable to a lineup task: The presence of duds (i.e., highly dissimilar fillers) may increase a witness’s confidence that an identified (non-dud) lineup member is the criminal. Four studies (N = 665) demonstrate that the mere presence of highly dissimilar fillers inflates witnesses’ confidence in a mistaken identification (Studies 1–4), provides evidence that this confidence inflation is due to the duds inflating the perceived similarity of the other lineup members to the criminal (Studies 2, 3), and delineates some conditions under which the effect holds (Studies 3, 4). The addition of highly dissimilar lineup members, far from being inert, as is often implicitly assumed, can bias witnesses’ confidence reports.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Consistent with the judgment and decision making literature, we use the term normative to refer to processes or judgments that are considered to fit a rational model or align with formal proofs, such as probability theory.

  2. Of course, the inclusion of duds could also theoretically increase witness confidence in a correct identification as well; however, the general point to be made is that dud-induced confidence inflation is a distortion of a witness’s ‘true’ confidence, since it derives from a source considered not relevant to the judgment.

References

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of the cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183–200. doi:10.1037/h0024835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York: Wiley. doi:10.1037/11622-000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, N., Gordon, M., & Bond, N. (2000). Effect of photoarray exposure duration on eyewitness identification accuracy and processing strategy. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 6, 21–32. doi:10.1080/10683160008410829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, S. D., Carlucci, M., Vallano, J., & Hyman Gregory, A. (2010). The selective cue integration framework: A theory of postidentification witness confidence assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 204–218. doi:10.1037/a0019495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, S. D., Hyman Gregory, A., & Carlucci, M. (2009). Exploring the diagnostic utility of facial composites: Beliefs of guilt can bias perceived similarity between composite and suspect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 76–90. doi:10.1037/a0014682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, S. D., & Wells, G. L. (2007). Applied lineup theory. In R. C. L. Lindsay, D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.) Handbook of eyewitness psychology: Memory for people (pp. 219–254). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Retrieved from www.csa.com.

  • Clark, S. E. (2003). A memory and decision model for eyewitness identification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 629–654. doi:10.1002/acp.891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, B., & Penrod, S. (1989). Forensically relevant moderators of the relation between eyewitness identification accuracy and confidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 650–652. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.74.4.650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, B., Penrod, S., & Dexter, H. (1990). Juror sensitivity to eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 14, 185–191. doi:10.1007/BF01062972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, B., Penrod, S., & Stuve, T. (1988). Juror decision making in eyewitness identification cases. Law and Human Behavior, 12, 41–55. doi:10.1007/BF01064273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglass, A. B., & Steblay, N. (2006). Memory distortion in eyewitnesses: A meta-analysis of the post-identification feedback effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 859–869. doi:10.1002/acp.1237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210. doi:10.1037/h0041593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2002). Testing the action-based model of cognitive dissonance: The effect of action orientation on postdecisional attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 711–723. doi:10.1177/0146167202289001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, N., & Förster, J. (2006). Inferences from decision difficulty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 290–301. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L., & Wells, G. L. (1980). What price justice? Exploring the relationship of lineup fairness to identification accuracy. Law and Human Behavior, 4, 303–313. doi:10.1007/BF01040622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L., Wells, G. L., & O’Connor, F. (1989). Mock juror belief of accurate and inaccurate eyewitnesses: A replication. Law and Human Behavior, 13, 333–340. doi:10.1007/BF01067033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malpass, R. S. (1981). Effective size and defendant bias in eyewitness identification lineups. Law and Human Behavior, 5, 299–309. doi:10.1007/BF01044945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malpass, R. S., & Devine, P. G. (1981). Eyewitness identification: Lineup instructions and the absence of the offender. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 482–489. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.66.4.482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McQuiston-Surrett, D., Douglass, A., & Burkhardt, S. G. (2008). Evaluation of facial composite evidence depends on the presence of other case factors. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 279–298. doi:10.1348/135532507X214192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meissner, C. A., Tredoux, C. J., Parker, J. F., & MacLin, O. H. (2005). Eyewitness decisions in simultaneous and sequential lineups. Memory & Cognition, 33, 783–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuschatz, J. S., Preston, E. L., Burkett, A. D., Toglia, M. R., Lampinen, J. M., Neuschatz, J. S., …Goodsell, C. A. (2005). The effects of post-identification feedback and age on retrospective eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 435–453. doi:10.1002/acp.1084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. S., III (1996). Increases in eyewitness confidence resulting from postevent questioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 126–146. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.2.2.126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. S., III, Appio, L. M., Zerr, T. K., & Pontoski, K. E. (2007). Public eyewitness confidence can be influenced by the presence of other witnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 629–652. doi:10.1007/s10979-006-9080-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, J. S., III, & McClure, K. A. (1996). Repeated postevent questioning can lead to elevated levels of eyewitness confidence. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 629–654. doi:10.1007/BF01499235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skagerberg, E. M. (2007). Co-witness feedback in line-ups. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 489–497. doi:10.1002/acp.1285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological Methodology 1982 (pp. 290–312). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sporer, S., Penrod, S., Read, D., & Cutler, B. L. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A meta-analysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 315–327. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.118.3.315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steblay, N. M. (1997). Social influence in eyewitness recall: A meta-analytic review of lineup instruction effects. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 283–298. doi:10.1023/A:1024890732059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence. (1999). Eyewitness evidence: A guide for law enforcement. Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Koehler, D. J. (1994). Support theory: A nonextensional representation of subjective probability. Psychological Review, 101, 547–567. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1998). “Good, you identified the suspect:” Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 360–376. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1999). Distortions in eyewitnesses’ recollections: Can the postidentification feedback effect be moderated? Psychological Science, 10, 138–144. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Ferguson, T. J., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1981). The tractability of eyewitness confidence and its implication for triers of fact. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 688–696. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.66.6.688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Leippe, M. R., & Ostrom, T. M. (1979). Guidelines for empirically assessing the fairness of a lineup. Law and Human Behavior, 3, 285–293. doi:10.1007/BF01039807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Lindsay, R. C. L., & Ferguson, T. J. (1979). Accuracy, confidence, and juror perceptions in eyewitness identification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 440–448. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.64.4.440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Murray, D. (1984). Eyewitness confidence. In G. L. Wells & E. F. Loftus (Eds.), Eyewitness testimony: Psychological perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Olson, E., & Charman, S. (2003). Distorted retrospective eyewitness reports as functions of feedback and delay. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 42–52. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.9.1.42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Rydell, S. M., & Seelau, E. P. (1993). On the selection of distractors for eyewitness lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 835–844. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.5.835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitley, B. E., Jr., & Greenberg, M. S. (1986). The role of eyewitness confidence in juror perceptions of credibility. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 387–409. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1986.tb01148.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windschitl, P. D., & Chambers, J. R. (2004). The dud-alternative effect in likelihood judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 198–215. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steve D. Charman.

Appendices

Appendix A

Dud lineup and actual perpetrator from Experiments 1 and 2. Note that the non-dud version of this lineup consisted of only lineup members 2 and 5.

figure a

Appendix B

Dud lineup and actual perpetrator from Experiment 3. Note that the non-dud lineup consisted of only lineup members 2 and 5.

figure b

Appendix C

Dud lineups and actual perpetrator from Experiment 4. Note that the non-dud version of the lineups consisted of lineup members 2 and 5.

figure c
figure d

About this article

Cite this article

Charman, S.D., Wells, G.L. & Joy, S.W. The Dud Effect: Adding Highly Dissimilar Fillers Increases Confidence in Lineup Identifications. Law Hum Behav 35, 479–500 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-010-9261-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-010-9261-1

Keywords

Navigation