Skip to main content
Log in

The Vertical Dimension of Social Relations and Accurate Interpersonal Perception: A Meta-Analysis

  • Review Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is little consensus regarding how verticality (social power, dominance, and status) is related to accurate interpersonal perception. The relation could be either positive or negative, and there could be many causal processes at play. The present article discusses the theoretical possibilities and presents a meta-analysis of this question. In studies using a standard test of interpersonal accuracy, higher socioeconomic status (SES) predicted higher accuracy defined as accurate inference about the meanings of cues; also, higher experimentally manipulated vertical position predicted higher accuracy defined as accurate recall of others’ words. In addition, although personality dominance did not predict accurate inference overall, the type of personality dominance did, such that empathic/responsible dominance had a positive relation and egoistic/aggressive dominance had a negative relation to accuracy. In studies involving live interaction, higher experimentally manipulated vertical position produced lower accuracy defined as accurate inference about cues; however, methodological problems place this result in doubt.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Henley (1977) tied this hypothesis to the established finding that women (considered to be low power) score higher on most interpersonal accuracy tests than men (considered to be high power) [for summaries of these gender differences see Hall (1984), or Hall and Gunnery (2013)].

  2. A small number of studies did not fit these categories and were not included in the meta-analytic summaries. Their verticality definitions were self-rating of dominance in one’s job, observer ratings of dominance during an interaction, influence that emerged in a group interaction according to peer or observer reports (emergent leadership), and self-reported power within one’s family. Another study was not included because though the power definition fitted our categories (personality dominance), it was the only personality study using recall as the accuracy measure. The effects for these left-out studies were small and inconsistent, with only two being significant but both showing that higher verticality was associated with greater interpersonal accuracy. In one, female medical students who scored higher on judging affective cues were rated by observers as more dominant when interacting with a standardized patient (Hall et al. 2009c). In the other, undergraduate business students who scored higher on judging affective cues were more likely to emerge as leaders within task groups to which they were assigned (Walter et al. 2012). Finally, although we initially conceptualized “status” as a broader topic than just SES, no studies using a broader definition (such as others’ respect) emerged in the literature search.

  3. A priori, we did not include studies of young children because it was not clear how directly applicable the verticality construct is to that age group. In fact, it was a moot point because there were few if any such studies.

  4. Although it is true that high sociometric status (i.e., popularity, peer regard) has implications for power and social influence in the peer group, the two are not synonymous. Popularity furthermore merges with other constructs that become increasingly tangential to the verticality construct, such as physical attractiveness, personality traits (e.g., extraversion), and general social competence. Previous research has found that accurate interpersonal perception is positively correlated with popularity and general social competence (Hall et al. 2009c).

  5. When the three studies in question were classified as being of the other type of dominance, and the results recalculated, little changed.

  6. This description is not what was given in the Kraus et al. (2010) article. The correct description was provided by personal communication from Kraus (May 9, 2011).

  7. Although meta-analysis has shown that training to improve interpersonal accuracy is effective, the specific mechanisms underlying the effectiveness of training are not well understood (Blanch-Hartigan et al. 2012).

References

Works marked with * contributed effect sizes to the reported analyses. T = testing paradigm, I = in vivo paradigm

  • Alkire, A. A., Collum, M. E., Kaswan, J., & Love, L. R. (1968). Information exchange and accuracy of verbal communication under social power conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 301–308. I

  • *Alvarez, G., & Fuentes, P. (1994). Recognition of facial expression in diverging socioeconomic levels. Brain and Cognition, 25, 235–239. T

  • Ambady, N., & Gray, H. M. (2002). On being sad and mistaken: Mood effects on the accuracy of thin-slice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 947–961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., & Skowronski, J. J. (Eds.). (2008). First impressions. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362–1377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Bachmann, M., & Schmid Mast, M. (2010). Unpublished data. T

  • *Bachmann, M., & Schmid Mast, M. (2011). Unpublished data. T

  • *Bailey, W., Nowicki, S., Jr., & Cole, S. P. (1998). The ability to decode nonverbal information in African American, African and Afro-Caribbean, and European American Adults. Journal of Black Psychology, 24, 418–431. T

  • *Barnes, M. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Social intelligence and decoding of nonverbal cues. Intelligence, 13, 263–287. T

  • *Barreto, M., Ellemers, N., & Fiske, S. T. (2010). “What did you say, and who do you think you are?” How power differences affect emotional reactions to prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 66, 477–492. T

  • Berdahl, J. L., & Martorana, P. (2006). Effects of power on emotion and expression during a controversial group discussion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 497–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J. (2001). Toward a taxonomy of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanch-Hartigan, D., Andrzejewski, S., & Hill, K. (2012). The effectiveness of training to improve person perception accuracy: A meta-analysis. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34, 483–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis (2nd ed.). Englewood, NJ: Biostat.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Boucher, E. M., Hancock, J. T., & Dunham, P. J. (2008). Interpersonal sensitivity in computer-mediated and face-to-face conversations. Media Psychology, 11, 235–258. I

  • Bugental, D. B. (2010). Paradoxical power manifestations: Power assertion by the subjectively powerless. In A. Guinote & T. K. Vescio (Eds.), The social psychology of power (pp. 209–230). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Byron, K. (2003). Are better managers better at “reading” others? Testing the claim that emotional intelligence predicts managerial performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA. T

  • Byron, K. (2007). Male and female managers’ ability to read emotions: Relationships with supervisor’s performance ratings and subordinates’ satisfaction ratings. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80, 713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byron, K., Terranova, S., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2007). Nonverbal emotion recognition and salespersons: Linking ability to perceived and actual success. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2600–2619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Carney, D. R. (2009). Unpublished data. T

  • *Casciaro, T. (1998). Seeing things clearly: Social structure, personality, and accuracy in social network perception. Social Networks, 20, 331–351. I

  • Chepenik, L. G., Cornew, L. A., & Farah, M. J. (2007). The influence of sad mood on cognition. Emotion, 7, 802–811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Cook, K. E. (2002). Target and perceiver gender in person perception: Power as a possible explanation for gender differences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, University of Washington. T

  • *Côté, S., Kraus, M. W., Cheng, B. H., Oveis, C., Van der Löwe, I., Lian, H., et al. (2011). Social power facilitates the effect of prosocial orientation on empathic accuracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 217–232. I, T

  • *Delgado-Hachey, M., & Miller, S. A. (1993). Mothers’ accuracy in predicting their children’s IQs: Its relationship to antecedent variables, mothers’ academic achievement demands, and children’s achievement. Journal of Experimental Education, 62, 43–59. I

  • Demenescu, L. R., Kortekaas, R., den Boer, J. A., & Aleman, A. (2010). Impaired attribution of emotion to facial expressions in anxiety and major depression. PLos ONE, 5, Article e15058.

  • DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Mead, N. L., & Vohs, K. D. (2011). How leaders self-regulate their task performance: Evidence that power promotes diligence, depletion, and disdain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 47–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiMatteo, M. R., Friedman, H. S., & Taranta, A. (1979). Sensitivity to bodily nonverbal communication as a factor in practitioner–patient rapport. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 18–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiMatteo, M. R., Hays, R. D., & Prince, L. M. (1986). Relationship of physicians’ nonverbal communication skill to patient satisfaction, appointment noncompliance, and physician workload. Health Psychology, 5, 581–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elfenbein, H. A., Foo, M. D., White, J., Tan, H. H., & Aik, V. C. (2007). Reading your counterpart: The benefit of emotion recognition accuracy for effectiveness in negotiation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 205–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellyson, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1985). Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior: Basic concepts and issues. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–27). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V. (1960). Interrelations among two dimensions of sociometric status, group congeniality and accuracy of social perception. Sociometry, 23, 85–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652–670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Funder, D. C., & Harris, M. J. (1986). On the several facets of personality assessment: The case of social acuity. Journal of Personality, 54, 528–550. T

  • Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not taken. Psychological Science, 17, 1068–1074. T

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gesn, P. R., & Ickes, W. (1999). The development of meaning contexts for empathic accuracy: Channel and sequence effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 746–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., & Ward, D. (2008). Power in mixed-sex stranger interactions. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1555–1568. I

  • *Gordon, A. M., & Chen, S. (2012). Unpublished data. I

  • Guinote, A. (2010). The situated focus theory of power. In A. Guinote & T. K. Vescio (Eds.), The social psychology of power (pp. 141–174). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guinote, A., & Vescio, T. K. (Eds.). (2010). The social psychology of power. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hall, J. A. (2001). Unpublished data. I

  • Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., & Yopchick, J. E. (2009a). Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Blanch, D. C., Horgan, T. G., Murphy, N. A., Rosip, J. C., & Schmid Mast, M. (2009b). Motivation and interpersonal sensitivity: Does it matter how hard you try? Motivation and Emotion, 33, 291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Hall, J. A., Carter, J. D., & Horgan, T. G. (2001). Status roles and recall of nonverbal cues. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 79–100. I

  • Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Gunnery, S. D. (2013). Gender differences in nonverbal communication. In J. A. Hall & M. L. Knapp (Eds.), Handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 639–669). Berlin: deGruyter Mouton.

  • *Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1994). “Subordination” and sensitivity to nonverbal cues: A study of married working women. Sex Roles, 31, 149–165. T

  • Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1997). Subordination and nonverbal sensitivity: A hypothesis in search of support. In M. R. Walsh (Ed.), Women, men, and gender: Ongoing debates (pp. 120–133). New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hall, J. A., Halberstadt, A. G., & O’Brien, C. E. (1997). “Subordination” and nonverbal sensitivity: A study and synthesis of findings based on trait measures. Sex Roles, 37, 295–317. T

  • Hall, J. A., & Schmid Mast, M. (2007). Sources of accuracy in the empathic accuracy paradigm. Emotion, 7, 438–446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Carney, D. R. (2006a). On the varieties of asymmetrical dependency: Feelings, motives, behavior, and accuracy in a dyadic interaction. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 583–599. I

  • Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Schmid Mast, M. (2006b). Recall of nonverbal cues: Exploring a new definition of interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 141–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Hall, J. A., Rosip, J. C., Smith LeBeau, L., Horgan, T. G., & Carter, J. D. (2006c). Attributing the sources of accuracy in unequal-power dyadic communication: Who is better and why? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 18–27. I

  • Hall, J. A., Roter, D. L., Blanch, D. C., & Frankel, R. M. (2009c). Nonverbal sensitivity in medical students: Implications for clinical interactions. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 24, 1217–1222.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hänggi, Y. (2004). Stress and emotion recognition: An internet experiment using stress induction. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 63, 113–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex, and nonverbal communication. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, L. R., Buckner, J. D., & Schmidt, N. B. (2009). Interpreting facial expressions: The influence of social anxiety, emotional valence, and race. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 482–488.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. D., & Bechler, C. (1998). Examining the relationship between listening effectiveness and leadership emergence: Perceptions, behaviors, and recall. Small Group Research, 29, 452–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Jowett, S., & Clark-Carter, D. (2006). Perceptions of empathic accuracy and assumed similarity in the coach–athlete relationship. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 617–637. I

  • Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, R., Garvin, J., Heaton, N., & Boyle, E. (2006). Emotional intelligence and leadership effectiveness. Leadership and Organization Development Journal, 27, 265–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kombos, N. A., & Fournet, G. P. (1985). Effects of dominance-submissiveness and gender on recognition of nonverbal emotional cues. Educational and Psychological Research, 5, 19–28. T

  • *Krackhardt, D. (1990). Assessing the political landscape: Structure, cognition, and power in organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35, 342–369. I

  • *Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–1723. I, T

  • Kraus, M. W., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., Rheinschmidt, M. L., & Keltner, D. (2012). Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review, 119, 546–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Kunstman, J. W., & Maner, J. K. (2011). Sexual overperception: Power, mating motives, and biases in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 282–294. I

  • LaFrance, M., & Henley, N. M. (1997). On oppressing hypotheses: Or, differences in nonverbal sensitivity revisited. In M. R. Walsh (Ed.), Women, men, and gender: Ongoing debates (pp. 104–119). New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lammers, J., Galinsky, A. D., Gordijn, E. H., & Otten, S. (2008). Illegitimacy moderates the effects of power on approach. Psychological Science, 19, 558–564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langner, C. A., & Keltner, D. (2008). Social power and emotional experience: Actor and partner effects within dyadic interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 848–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Letzring, T. D. (2008). The good judge of personality: Characteristics, behaviors, and observer accuracy. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 914–932. I

  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, R. G., & Hall, R. J. (2005). Identity, deep structure and the development of leadership skill. Leadership Quarterly, 16, 591–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Lord, C. G., & Saenz, D. S. (1985). Memory deficits and memory surfeits: Differential cognitive consequences of tokenism for tokens and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 918–926. T

  • Magee, J. C., & Smith, P. K. (2013). The social distance theory of power. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 158–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., Butz, D. A., & Peruche, B. M. (2007). Power, risk, and the status quo: Does power promote riskier or more conservative decision making? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 451–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, D., Slessor, G., Allen, R., Phillips, L. H., & Darling, S. (2012). Processing orientation and emotion recognition. Emotion, 12, 39–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *McDonough, M. (1992). Gender and perceived power as predictors of accuracy in nonverbal decoding. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology. T

  • *Mill, A., Allik, J., Realo, A., & Valk, R. (2009). Age-related differences in emotion recognition ability: A cross-sectional study. Emotion, 9, 619–630. T

  • *Moeller, S. K., Lee, E. A. E., & Robinson, M. D. (2011). You never think about my feelings: Interpersonal dominance as a predictor of emotion decoding accuracy. Emotion, 11, 816–824. T

  • Mumford, T. V., Campion, M. A., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). The leadership skills strataplex: Leadership skill requirements across organizational levels. Leadership Quarterly, 18, 154–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, N. A., & Hall, J. A. (2011). Intelligence and nonverbal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 39, 54–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noller, P. (1980). Misunderstandings in marital communication: A study of couples’ nonverbal communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1135–1148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Overbeck, J. R., & Park, B. (2001). When power does not corrupt: Superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 549–565. T

  • Overbeck, J. R., & Park, B. (2006). Powerful perceivers, powerless objects: Flexibility of powerholders’ social attention. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 227–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1995). A parallel process model of nonverbal communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Paulmann, S. (2011). Unpublished data. T

  • *Pfaff, P. L. (1954). An experimental study of the communication of feeling without contextual material. Speech Monographs, 21, 155. T

  • Phillips, L. H., Channon, S., Tunstall, M., Hedenstrom, A., & Lyons, K. (2008). The role of working memory in decoding emotions. Emotion, 8, 184–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L. H., Tunstall, M., & Channon, S. (2007). Exploring the role of working memory in dynamic social cue decoding using dual task methodology. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 137–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W. (2009). Analyzing effect sizes: Random-effects models. In H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges, & J. C. Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed., pp. 295–315). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E. (2001). Interpersonal sensitivity research and organizational psychology: Theoretical and methodological applications. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Intepersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 305–317). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 638–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 183–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. T

  • Rosete, D., & Ciarrochi, J. (2005). Emotional intelligence and its relationship to workplace performance outcomes of leadership effectiveness. Leadership and Organization Development Journal, 26, 388–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Rosip, J. C. (2006). Encoding and decoding ability in unequal-status dyadic communication: Status/power, emotion, and comfort. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northeastern University, Boston, MA.

  • Rubin, R. S., Munz, D. C., & Bommer, W. H. (2005). Leading from within: The effects of emotion recognition and personality on transformational leadership behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 845–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, A. M., & Fiske, S. T. (2010). Power and social perception. In A. Guinote & T. K. Vescio (Eds.), The social psychology of power (pp. 231–250). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Sabatelli, R. M., Buck, R., & Dreyer, A. (1982). Nonverbal communication accuracy in married couples: Relationship with marital complaints. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1088–1097. T

  • *Saenz, D. S., & Lord, C. G. (1989). Reversing roles: A cognitive strategy for undoing memory deficits associated with token status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 698–708. T

  • *Sasson, N. J., Pinkham, A. E., Richard, J., Hughett, P., Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (2010). Controlling for response biases clarifies sex and age differences in facial affect recognition. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34, 207–221. T

  • *Scherer, K. R., & Scherer, U. (2011). Assessing the ability to recognize facial and vocal expressions of emotion: Construction and validation of the Emotion Recognition Index. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 305–326. T

  • Schmid, P. C., Schmid Mast, M., Bombari, D., & Mast, F. W. (2011a). Gender effects in information processing on a nonverbal decoding task. Sex Roles, 65, 102–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, P. C., Schmid Mast, M., Bombari, D., Mast, F. W., & Lobmaier, J. (2011b). How mood states affect information processing during facial emotion recognition: An eye tracking study. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70, 223–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Schmid Mast, M. (2008). Unpublished data. T

  • Schmid Mast, M. (2010). Interpersonal behavior and social perception in a hierarchy: The Interpersonal Power and Behaviour Model. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Schmid Mast, M., & Darioly, A. (2014). Emotion recognition accuracy in hierarchical relationships. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 73, 69–75. T

  • Schmid Mast, M., Jonas, K., Cronauer, C. K., & Darioly, A. (2012). On the importance of the superior’s interpersonal sensitivity for good leadership. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 1043–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Schmid Mast, M., Jonas, K., & Hall, J. A. (2009). Give a person power and he or she will show interpersonal sensitivity: The phenomenon and its why and when. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 835–850. T

  • Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., van Laar, C., & Levin, S. (2004). Social dominance theory: Its agenda and method. Political Psychology, 25, 845–880.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Simpson, B., & Borch, C. (2005). Does power affect perception in social networks? Two arguments and an experimental test. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 278. T

  • Smith, P. K., Wigboldus, D. H. J., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2008). Abstract thinking increases one’s sense of power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 378–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Snodgrass, S. E. (1985). Women’s intuition: The effect of subordinate role on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 146–155. I

  • *Snodgrass, S. E. (1992). Further effects of role versus gender on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 154–158. I

  • Snodgrass, S. E., Hecht, M. A., & Ploutz-Snyder, R. (1998). Interpersonal sensitivity: Expressivity or perceptivity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 238–249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Stokes, D. R. (1983). Nonverbal communication: Race, gender, social class, world view and the PONS test; Implications for the therapeutic dyad. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University. T

  • *Thomas, D. L., Franks, D. D., & Calonico, J. M. (1972). Role-taking and power in social psychology. American Sociological Review, 37, 605–614. I

  • Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008). The automaticity of emotion recognition. Emotion, 8, 81–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Trefry, M. G. (1991). Power and perspective-taking: The perspective-taking accuracy of employees and managers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University. I

  • Walter, F., Cole, M. S., van der Vegt, G. S., Rubin, R. S., & Bommer, W. H. (2012). Emotion recognition and emergent leadership: Unraveling mediating mechanisms and boundary conditions. Leadership Quarterly, 23, 977–991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Magali Ecabert for her assistance in coding, and the authors who provided their unpublished results.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judith A. Hall.

Appendix: Coded Study Attributes

Appendix: Coded Study Attributes

Year of study

Gender of first author

Sample type (college/university students, hereafter referred to as college students, employees in organization, community people, or mix of groups)

Sample age (mean or median) and sample age group (13–17, 18–22, 23–27, 28–32, 33–37, 38–42, 43 and older, or mixed ages)

Sample size

Percentage of participants who were male

Type of design/analysis (experimental manipulation, known groups comparison, or continuous correlation)

Type of comparison (within-dyads comparison between higher and lower vertical partners or between-groups comparison of groups that differed in verticality)

Location (laboratory or field)

Verticality definition (role-played/assigned high and low vertical roles/imagined vertical position, measured hierarchical position in organization, measured hierarchical position in group, self-reported dominant personality, other-reported dominant personality, behavioral measurement, SES, psychological induction other than assigned roles and if yes, was it concept priming or autobiographical writing, self-reported feelings of verticality in or after a situation, being minority or majority within a group, or self-reported power vis-à-vis other family members)

If personality dominance was measured, a moderator was coded to capture the type of dominance scale (egoistic/aggressive or empathic/responsible).

Accuracy paradigm (testing paradigm or in vivo)

Accuracy method (test, recall, correlation between perceiver and target ratings, or difference between perceiver and target ratings)

If a test was used, which one: PONS (Rosenthal et al. 1979), DANVA (Nowicki and Duke 1994), or other

If a test was given, content of accuracy inference (affect/emotion, personality, other’s thoughts/feelings about self and partner, or other)

If recall was measured, content of recall (words, nonverbal cues or appearance, or other).

Additional coding was done to estimate, from an observer’s perspective, the psychological states that the higher and lower vertical people in each study might have been experiencing. Two coders who were blind to the studies’ results (IML and a research assistant) performed ratings of psychological states based on the studies’ methodologies, using two items for each of four constructs that were rated on nine-point unipolar scales: (1) prosocial: helpful/cooperative, attentive to others’ feelings or thoughts, (2) self-centered: egocentric/concerned with own needs, wanting to promote self/self-aggrandizing, (3) angry affect: angry/irritated, cheerful/pleasant (reversed), and (4) anxious affect: uncomfortable/awkward, fearful/worried. The two coders’ ratings were averaged, and the two items belonging to each construct were averaged. These combined ratings were then correlated with the effect sizes separately for the higher and lower vertical participants and also after subtracting the ratings of the lower participants from the ratings of the higher participants on each moderator, thus creating difference scores reflecting the disparity in psychological states. Finally, given the predictions made by the social distance theory of power (Magee and Smith 2013), namely that high power results in less interpersonal accuracy (because high power results in feeling more distant and less similar to social interaction partners and empathic accuracy is reduced between dissimilar interaction partners), the two coders also directly rated the verticality gap (social distance) between the higher and lower individuals on a 9-point scale that went from only a small gap to a very big gap. Correlations between these variables and effect sizes were few and inconsistent. Therefore, these moderator ratings are not discussed further.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hall, J.A., Schmid Mast, M. & Latu, IM. The Vertical Dimension of Social Relations and Accurate Interpersonal Perception: A Meta-Analysis. J Nonverbal Behav 39, 131–163 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0205-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0205-1

Keywords

Navigation