1 Introduction

Research to date on subjective satisfaction in Europe and Eurasia has focused mainly on the European Union, including the former communist countries of Central and Eastern Europe that have now joined the EU. With the notable exception of the Russian Federation much less attention has been paid to the former Soviet Union (FSU) and virtually none to the Central Asia Republics and the countries of the Caucuses. However, the development trajectories of the countries of Eastern and Central Europe (CEE) have been very different from those of the Commonwealth of Independent States. Most of the former had entered on a ‘virtuous’ path of economic, political and civic development by the start of the twenty-first century, with reported levels of subjective satisfaction improving (Abbott and Wallace 2009a, b, c; Easterlin 2009; Wallace and Haerpfer 2002). The length and depth of the economic recession was much greater and recovery much slower in the successor states to the Soviet Union and the future more uncertain. There has not been the same move towards the establishment of regulated market economies, democratic government and the development of civil society found in the countries of Eastern and Central Europe (Grun and Klasen 2000; Haerpfer 2009).

It is now well established that the disintegration of the Soviet Union in 1991 has had a negative impact on the health, wealth and well-being of the population. The social transformation and structural change, backed and fuelled by economic collapse, led to a breakdown of the normative structure and predictability of the social order—what Durkheim referred to as ‘anomie (Abbott and Beck 2003; Durkheim 1952; Galip 2009; Genov 1998; Krivosheyev 2004; Pridemore et al. 2007). Much of the research on the impact of the post-1991 structural transformation on the well-being of the societies and the citizens of the former USSR has focused on the Russian Federation and has demonstrated that the changes have had a dramatically negative impact on the welfare of the societies and the population (see e.g. Abbott 2007; Abbott and Sapsford 2006; Abbott and Wallace 2007, 2009a; Burawoy 1997, 2000, 2001; Burawoy et al. 2000; Shevchenko 2009; Rose 2009; Wallace and Abbott 2009). However, much less is known about the impact of the transformation on the life-satisfaction of those who have survived in other CIS countries, including the Central Asian Republics and the Caucasus, although the very limited research, mostly looking at the Central Asian Republics, suggests that there has been a negative impact (Abbott 2002; Abbott and Wallace 2009a; Cockerham et al. 2004; De la Sablonniere et al. 2009; Galip 2009; Kanji 2007; Namazie and Sandfrey 2002; Nazpary 2002; Richardson et al. 2008; Wallace and Abbott 2009). An index of well-being for children, developed for 21 CEE and CIS countries (excluding the CEE countries that joined the EU in 2003) and using a range of objective and subjective indicators, suggests that the four countries considered here are below the regional average in this respect, with Kazakhstan being marginally better than the other three countries (Richardson et al. 2008).

The data we use are about individuals, but the focus of the paper is on the nature of the societies in which they live—what makes a society more or less tolerable, gives more or less space for citizens to take control of their lives. Satisfaction is used as an indicator of the good society—people will necessarily be dissatisfied with intolerable societies, and even where people have come to terms with societies which are far from optimal, their level of satisfaction is likely to be lower than in societies which give a better than tolerable life-style. The focus of the paper is not individual satisfaction per se, however, but what it is about societies that makes satisfaction possible.

2 Background

The “shock therapy” that resulted in system disintegration of the political and economic structures in the FSU was unprecedented in modern times. It resulted not only in systems disintegration but also in social disintegration, with people having to negotiate between old familiar practices and the new realities which faced them in their daily lives. People experienced the transformation as living in a permanent state of chaos/crisis (Nazpary 2002; Shevchenko 2009) with no known outcome. The transformation involved four interdependent processes:

  • the construction of new state formations and related institutions;

  • a shift from planned and administered co-ordination of markets to economic markets and private property;

  • the move from the hegemony of the Communist Party to authoritarian regimes; and

  • a realignment of states in the arena of international relations.

All aspects of social, economic and political life changed at once, and it was a painful process for the majority of the population. New states emerged that lacked the institutional mechanisms for social integration and social stability (Fligstein 2001; Walder 1994). New class relationships were unleashed, and there was an intense struggle to secure access to and control over resources. The transition from a bureaucratic-redistributive order to an imperfect market order was accompanied by a dramatic decline in GDP, a decline in state spending on health, education and social security benefits both in absolute and real terms, an increase in unemployment and under-employment, the non- and late payment of wages, a decline in the influence of the trade unions, an increase in inequalities, poverty and malnutrition and a growth in informal economic activity (Abbott 2002). The vast majority of the population were losers (Haerpfer and Zeilhofer 1995; Rose 2009) with survival for many depending on participating in informal economies in place of, or as well as, the formal economy. Those with know-how—the “winners”—were able to exploit this situation, but the majority of the population were left unable to participate (Rose 2009).

The transformation inflicted considerable stress on the population (Wallace and Abbott 2009) with the breakdown of social order evidenced by an increase in lawlessness, a dramatic growth in social inequalities and the inability of the public powers to regulate the emerging market economy or to exercise good governance, to win the people’s trust or to take care of the losers (Ledeneva 2006; Rose 2009; Sapsford and Abbott 2006). With weakened generalised trust—a loss of trust in the impersonal institutions necessary for the effective working of parties, business and civil society—the response of many was to retreat into defensive anti-modern modes of survival relying on informal economic activity and on kin (Abbott and Wallace 2009a; Burawoy 2000; Rose 2009). The basis for citizenship was destroyed (Giddens 1994) and the security of the lives of the majority of the population was shattered (Abbott and Wallace 2007, 2009a; Rose 2009). The dislocation in the social structure resulted in a breakdown in the normative patterns that define the expectation of actors, in the patterns of social relationships between actors and in the embodied perceptions, habits and skills by which people produce and reproduce institutional and related structures. The structural change means not only that people’s life chances have been transformed (and, for many, for the worse) but that their understandings of how to make life choices and their ability actually to do so have also changed. Culturally shared templates are no longer appropriate for guiding behaviours in the changed socio-economic and cultural contexts (Sztompka 2002; Abbott and Wallace 2009a).

The four countries under consideration in this paper declared their independence from the former Soviet Union in December 1991 and joined the loosely confederated Commonwealth of Independent States (CIS) at that time. Kyrgyzstan, Georgia and Armenia are three of the poorest CIS countries and are highly dependent on their agricultural sector, whilst Kazakhstan has more developed industrial and service sectors mainly based on the exploitation of its natural resources including oil. (Abbott 2002; EastAgri 2009). All four have become authoritarian regimes (Haerpfer 2009). All the countries experienced hyperinflation and an economic collapse in the 1990s, but by 2001 inflation was generally under control and there were some signs of economic recovery. In Kazakhstan the adjusted real GDP (PPPUS$) exceed that of 1990 by 2001, but in the other three countries it was still significantly below the 1990 level. All the countries experienced significant growth in income inequalities over the 1990s (Abbott 2002) and poverty levels were high in 2001 (Table 1). There was a reliance on informal economic activity, with households often relying on a portfolio of incomes, especially in Armenia, Georgia and Kyrgyzstan (Abbott 2002; Abbott and Wallace 2009a). The health of the population was generally poor, although, with the notable exception of Kazakhstan, they did not experience the same sharp increase in mortality amongst men in mid-life that was experienced in the CIS countries of Eastern European and Russia (Abbott 2002; Cockerham et al. 2004; Wallace and Abbott 2009).

Table 1 Adjusted Real GDP 1990 and 2001 (PPPUS$) and incidence of poverty 1999–2000

Armenia is a landlocked country in the Caucus region of South West Asia, bordered by Turkey to the West, Georgia to the North, Azerbaijan to the East and Iran to the South. It remains heavily dependent on its Diaspora for economic survival and there has been significant outmigration. In 2001 the population was estimated to be around four million, but the correct figure is thought to be at least a million lower. Thirty-six per cent of the population live in rural areas, and in 1999–2000 just over 40 per cent of the population lived below the US$2 a day poverty line—the absolute poverty line for this region recommended by the World Bank (2000)—and over four-fifths below the US$4 poverty line recommended by Milanovic (1998) as a more realistic measure for this region.

Georgia is located in the Caucasus in South West Asia, bordering on the Black sea between Turkey and Russia. It had an estimated population of just fewer than five million in 2001, although the correct figure is thought to be a million lower than this. Forty-eight per cent of the population live in rural areas. In 1999–2000 just under a fifth of the population lived below the US$2 poverty line and just over half below the US$4 poverty line.

Kazakhstan is the largest of the Central Asian countries, with a landmass about the size of the European Union. It is landlocked and shares a long northern border with Russia, the Caspian Sea and Russia are to the west, China is to the east, and Kyrgyzstan, Uzbekistan, and Turkmenistan lie to the south. It had an estimated population in 2001 of just fewer than 15 million, 52 per cent Kazakh, with ethnic Russians—numbering about five million—constituting the second largest ethnic group. Some 1.3 million Russians, including many of the most educated people, left the country between 1990 and 1998. It has significant natural resources and these have fuelled the economic recovery. Fifty-six per cent of population live in rural areas. Some 15.7 per cent of the population lived below the $2 per day and 31.6 per cent below the US$4 per day poverty line in 1999–2000.

Kyrgyzstan is small and poor, with agriculture dominating the economy and contributing a third to GDP. It is landlocked; Kazakhstan lies to the north and China to the east, and the country borders Tajikistan and Uzbekistan in the south and west. Just over two-thirds of the population live in rural areas, and around 60 per cent of all workers are employed in agriculture (mainly as subsistence producers). It is predominately Sunni Muslim, although Islam is not a dominant force. The population in 2001 numbered 4.9 million, with about 60 per cent native Kyrgyz. The remainder are largely Russians and Uzbeks. In 1999–2000 half the population lived below the US$2 per day poverty line and nearly nine tenths below the US$4 per day poverty line.

3 Quality of Life

There are multiple definitions of quality of life, both based on objective and subjective criteria and a multiplicity of ways in which people asses their own quality of life or subjective well-being. However, an individual’s quality of life depends not only on what they do but also on what happens in the wider society and the impact this has on individuals differentially placed in the societal opportunity structure.

Objective and subjective indicators of satisfaction or well-being correlate highly (Economist Intelligence Unit 2005; Veenhoven 2009) but objective indicators such as GDP, UNDP HDI and IMR do not tell us what makes a society liveable or tolerable. It is now widely recognised, however, that people are good judges of their own circumstances and that reported subjective well-being (e.g. general satisfaction) reflects people’s lived experience and is meaningful (Clark et al. 2006; Kahneman and Krueger 2006 Land et al. 2007; Easterlin 2008). In this paper we consider the influences on general satisfaction, which is seen for the most part as a relatively stable cognitive evaluation circumstances strongly influenced by material situation (Meadow et al. 1992). Research on subjective satisfaction uses a variety of data sets that do not provide the same variables for analysis; a wide range of different indicators are used by researchers and the justification for the selection of indicators is not always clear. In selecting indicators Allardt’s classification of having (material factors), loving (social support) and being (sense of overall recognition) has, however, been important (see Allardt 1993; Bohnke 2005). The most frequently used dependent variable is the answer to a question on satisfaction with life in general (some research uses the answer to a question on happiness) but the wording of the question varies as does the range of possible answers. The selection of indicators, not surprisingly, influences what is found to influence subjective well-being, although there is also a good deal of consistency in terms of the most important indicators. It is also likely that responses to the question on general satisfaction will be influenced by the questions preceding it in the survey.

4 General Satisfaction in the CIS and Europe

There are wide differences in social and individual well-being across Europe and the former Soviet Union. Generally the pattern is a North–South, West-East gradient, with the countries of Western Europe having the high levels of social well-being and the most satisfied and happy citizens and those of Eastern Europe and the former Soviet Union being the least happy and satisfied (e.g. Abbott 2007; Abbott and Sapsford 2006; Abbott and Wallace 2009b, c; Andren and Martinsson 2009; Bohnke 2005; Delhey 2004; Veenhoven 2001). There is some evidence that levels of general satisfaction fell following the fall of communism but that by the turn of the twenty-first century they had recovered somewhat (Easterlin 2009).

In 2003 the mean level of subjective general satisfaction in the EU 15 was 7.4 on a ten-point scale, varying from a low of 5.9 in Portugal—above the mid-point of the scale (5.5)—to a high of 8.4 in Denmark. For the new member states and candidate countries of Central and Eastern Europe the mean score was 5.8, varying from a low of 4.4 in Bulgaria to a high of 7.0 in Slovenia. The means for the three new member states that had formed part of the FSU until 1991 were: Estonia 5.8; Latvia 5.5, and; Lithuania 5.3—around the mid-point of the scale. In 2001 the average level of general satisfaction in the CIS was 2.4 on a four-point scale—below the mid-point of 2.5—varying from a low of 1.6 in Georgia (notably lower than any of the other countries) to a high of 2.7 in Kyrgyzstan, above the mid-point but not by much (see Table 2). In four of the CIS countries the mean was below the mid-point and in three around it. This suggests that levels of general subjective satisfaction are somewhat lower in the former Soviet Union than in the former communist countries of Eastern and Central Europe but that the differences are not large.

Table 2 General satisfaction CIS in 2001 and central and eastern Europe in 2003

5 Explaining General Satisfaction in the CIS and Europe

Material circumstances tend to be the main predictor of subjective satisfaction, with age and gender making little difference, at least in Europe and the CIS (Abbott and Wallace 2009b, c; Andren and Martinsson 2009; Bohnke 2005; Delhey 2004). However, the perceived quality of the society also has an independent influence, especially on satisfaction (Bohnke 2005, 2008), as does social support, which generally becomes more important the higher the level of prosperity of the country, with family support being more important in poorer countries and friends in more affluent countries. The general pattern is that the lower the level of the GDP of a country, the lower the level of satisfaction throughout the population, with a greater variance in general satisfaction in poorer countries than in more affluent ones. Within countries material circumstances are generally the main or a major factor, although they tend to be less important in more affluent countries. A number of other factors, objective and subjective, have also been shown to influence subjective satisfaction fairly consistently In the CIS, in addition to economic circumstances, trust, social support (having friends and family to rely on), social inclusion (e.g. being in employment, being married, belonging to a civil society organisation), health and feeling in control of one’s life have been shown to contribute to explaining general satisfaction (Abbott 2007; Abbott and Sapsford 2006; Galip 2009; Kanji 2007; Namazie and Sandfrey 2002). Age, gender, education and employment status generally have only a small impact.

Using a sociologically informed approach and selecting indicators derived from the Social Quality model (Phillips 2006), Abbott and Wallace (2009b, c) found that the main factors influencing subjective satisfaction across the EU 27 in 2003 and 2007 were: having an adequate income for a decent standard of living, trusting other people and trusting government, having close support and feeling socially integrated, and being healthy and feeling in control of one’s life. They also found that economic factors were more important in the former communist countries of Central and Eastern Europe than in the EU 15, with some indication that as the economies of the former recovered, so the importance of economic factors declined somewhat, with social support and feeling in control of one’s life becoming more important. Age, gender education made little contribution to explaining differences.

6 Developing a Sociological Model of Subjective Satisfaction

As sociologists we are concerned to derive our indicators from a sociological base. We want to be able to define the type of society and social policies that enable people to have a satisfactory or at least tolerable life. To do this we have to consider the articulation between the quality of society and the subjective quality of individuals’ lives within it (e.g. Abbott 2007; Abbott and Sapsford 2006; Abbott and Wallace 2007; Bohnke 2005; Phillips 2006; Veenhoven 2008). We have to go beyond a description of objective living conditions and take account of citizens’ subjective understanding of their life situation and the extent to which they feel able to make the necessary choices in order to act to secure their well-being—to choose a style of life they value (Phillips 2006; Abbott and Wallace 2009b, c).

Subjective satisfaction has to be seen as the ultimate validating measure, on the argument that people should be more satisfied in a tolerable than an intolerable society (Land et al. 2007; Richardson et al. 2008). This is not a paper about subjective satisfaction; however, in the same way that Durkheim’s Suicide (1952) was not a treatise about suicide. What Durkheim was exploring was the nature of anomic societies—societies where the rules have broken down and there are no clear prescriptions for action—using suicide as a measurable indicator of the extent to which societies were anomic and as validation of his theory-based conclusions. What we are trying to do here, similarly, is to explore the effects of the various breakdowns—economic, political, social and normative/ideological—which were occasioned by the breakdown of the Soviet socio-political system. The concern is with how societies have rebuilt themselves—or not—into social spaces (see Abbott and Sapsford 2005) which are tolerable or even satisfactory as human habitats.

To relate agency and structure we need to take a sociological approach and go beyond considering people’s individual satisfaction with their life as a whole and with domains of satisfaction, to consider the relationship between objective conditions and the subjective evaluation of them and the impact this has on people’s ability to take control over their lives (Phillips 2006; Wallace and Abbott 2009)—in other words, to consider the material and the discursive nature of the society in which they live. The Social Quality approach, which focuses on the individual as an active subject living in developing social conditions, enables us to theorise the ways in which social and system integration impact on well-being. It measures the extent to which the quality of daily life provides for an acceptable standard of living, taking account of the structural features of societies and their institutions as assessed by reference to their impact on citizens. Social Quality defines the space within which citizens are able to participate in the social and economic life of their communities under conditions which enhance their well-being and individual potential. It is the social context within which individuals have the opportunity to develop the capabilities to enable them to attain valued outcomes (Sen 1993). It identifies four domains or areas—economic security (necessary material resources), social cohesion (necessary accepted norms and values in place), social inclusion (access to necessary institutional and infrastructural context) and the conditions for empowerment (the extent to which people feel they have control over their own lives and the capacity for meaningful agency). Each of the four aspects has been shown to make an independent contribution to explaining satisfaction in other CIS countries (Abbott and Sapsford 2006; Abbott 2007) and the European Union (Abbott and Wallace 2009b, c).

We build on these insights to consider what range of factors influences well-being in Kazakhstan, Kyrgyzstan, Armenia and Georgia, in order to cast further light on the differential impact of the transition on post–Soviet citizens. These four countries are of interest particularly because they are not part of the developed West, for which the Social Quality model was first developed, and show that the model has generality beyond the countries for which it was first devised.

7 Methods

This paper dawns on a survey carried out in 2001 as part of The Living Conditions, Lifestyles and Health Project. Samples of 2,000 were selected in each country using multi-stage sampling with stratification by region and area. There was no over- or under-sampling of subgroups; however, in Georgia the regions of Abhazhia and Osetia had to be excluded because of inaccessibility (3% of the national population). Within each primary sampling unit households were selected using standardized random route procedures, except in Armenia, where systematic random sampling from a list was used. The response rates were 82 per cent in Kazakhstan, 71 per cent in Kyrgyzstan and 88 per cent in both Armenia and Georgia. The questionnaire was developed by the project partners and translated into Russian. Following a pilot it was amended and back translated into English for final checking.

Given the need to consider the multiple influences on the dependent variables a series of OLS regressions were carried out: for each quadrant of the social quality model and then for all the significant indicators with the dependent variable being general satisfaction.—The responses to the question How satisfied are you, all things considered, with your life as a whole these days1 definitely satisfied, 2 quite satisfied, 3 rather dissatisfied or 4 definitely dissatisfied?

The independent variables were selected from a rich data source as indicators (approximate measures) of the elements of our model (see below and “Appendix”): Material Circumstances, Social Cohesion, Social integration and Conditions for Social Empowerment. We also control for age and gender. When the countries were entered as dummy variables with Kazakhstan as reference the adjusted R 2 increased significantly and the country Betas were strong, suggesting that the model may not be entirely a general one but conceal some degree of difference between countries. Given this, the regression analysis was re-run for each country separately.

Nine of the independent variables were scales/indices computed for use in the analysis (social resource, personal support, trust in government, trust in institutions, personal control, malaise, freedom, fear of crime and political influence—see “Appendix”). The scales were constructed using factor analysis with varimax rotation and all had acceptable Cronbach alpha values. The reason for adding answers to several questions into a single scales were (1) to combine items which theory would regard as aspects of the same underlying/latent variable, and (2) to increase reliability by averaging out random measurement error. Often it also had the effect of increasing the variability of a measure, and so making it more useful in regression analysis.

All of the independent variables were tested and found to correlate with the dependent variable. The models were tested for multicollinearity and were found to be satisfactory as the tolerance of no variable was below 0 .40 (Tarling 2009). No variable was unacceptably skewed or platy/leptokurtic.

8 General Satisfaction

It is perhaps not surprising that the citizens of the four countries report relatively low levels of general satisfaction: only 12.3 per cent in Georgia, 41.5 per cent in Armenia, 57.5 per cent in Kazakhstan and 61.1 per cent in Kyrgyzstan said they were definitely satisfied or fairly satisfied. On a four point scale the means were, Georgia 1.6, Armenia, 2.3, Kazakhstan 2.6 and Kyrgyzstan 2.7 (Table 2). What is perhaps surprising, in this and other studies (e.g. Richardson et al. 2008), is that the highest levels of general satisfaction are found in Kyrgyzstan, the poorest country, and a much lower level of general satisfaction in Georgia compared with the other three countries. We return to this in the discussion below.

9 Economic Circumstances

As we have already shown (Table 1) there is clear evidence of the negative impact of the transformation on the living conditions of the populations of the four countries. A majority of respondents to our survey interpreted the post-1991 changes as having had a negative impact on their society and their own lives; 70 per cent of respondents in Kazakhstan, over 80 per cent in Kyrgyzstan, 91 per cent in Georgia and 96 per cent in Armenian thought that the disintegration of the USSR had had a negative impact on the economy of their country, for example. Whilst a substantial majority rated the economy as having been sound in the past (72.9% in Kazakhstan, 90% in Kyrgyzstan, 91.8% in Armenia, 92.2% in Georgia), few thought it was sound in 2001 (15.9 in Kazakhstan, 9.5 in Kyrgyzstan, 3.6 in Armenia, 1.1 in Georgia). The majority of respondents were dissatisfied with the material situation of their family: 58.4 per cent in Kyrgyzstan, 73.8 per cent in Kazakhstan, 83.3 per cent in Armenia and 91.8 per cent in Georgia. Satisfaction with financial situation has been shown to correlate highly with general satisfaction. We found this to be the case amongst our respondents, with the correlation being 0.48 in Armenia, 0.59 in Kazakhstan, 0.66 in Kirgizstan and 0.72 in Georgia.

The reported current material circumstances of our respondents suggest high levels of poverty, with only three and a half per cent in Armenia and two and a half per cent in Georgia reporting the economic circumstances of their households as good and with around a fifth of those in Kazakhstan (16.3%) and Kyrgyzstan (20.7%) doing so. Over three quarters of informants in Armenia (75.5%) and Kyrgyzstan (84.3%), two-thirds in Georgia (68.4%) and just under a half in Kazakhstan (43%), said that they are unable or only just able to afford to purchase necessary food. Indeed, over a third of respondents in Armenia (35.4%) and Kyrgyzstan (33.1%) said that they could never afford essential food, with around a tenth in Georgia (8.8%) and Kazakhstan (12.1%). This suggests high levels of relative and absolute poverty in the four countries, with higher levels of poverty in Armenia and Kyrgyzstan and with Kazakhstan having the lowest levels of poverty. It is worth noting that despite the high levels of reported poverty in Kyrgyzstan the levels of dissatisfaction with economic circumstances was lower than in the other three countries and we shall return to this (Table 3).

Table 3 Economic circumstances

We have selected three indicators of the economic situation of our respondents:

  1. 1.

    The economic situation of the household ranging, from very good to very bad on a five point scale. This measures where individuals place their household’s economic situation compared to others—a relative measure;

  2. 2.

    The ability of the household to purchase a range of goods and services ranging from basic household items to major consumer goods such as a car. This provides a material living conditions/deprivation scale and is comparable across the countries;

  3. 3.

    The ability to afford essential food most of the time, coded 0 or 1. This is a measure of absolute poverty.

(We should note that in countries with high levels of earnings from informal economies it is difficult to measure income.)

The three selected economic indicators together explain 23.6 per cent of the variance in general satisfaction (Table 7). The variables that make a significant contribution are the economic situation of the family and material living conditions, suggesting that general satisfaction increases as material circumstances increase.

10 Social Cohesion

Social cohesion, as the term is used in the Social Quality model, is the extent to which a society can be seen as a single integrated entity at the discursive level, with shared norms and values. One of the best indicators of social cohesion is trust (Phillips 2006). Trust tends to be highest amongst those who think there are few social conflicts, where people feel safe and where there are strong informal networks (Delhey and Newton 2003). Levels of trust are generally low in these four countries (Table 4, and see Sapsford and Abbott 2006). Overall levels of trust are highest in Kyrgyzstan, followed by Kazakhstan and Armenia, with Georgia having on average the lowest levels of trust. It is especially notable that general trust in other people (one indicator, perhaps, of feeling that the society is ‘pulling together’ around a single set of norms and values) is much higher in Kyrgyzstan than in the other three countries—although so is trust in organs of government and formal institutions. However, fear of crime (which is one form of distrust of fellow-citizens) is notably higher in Kyrgyzstan along with Kazakhstan and lower in Armenia.

Table 4 Social cohesion

We selected four indicators as measures of social cohesion:

  1. 1.

    The answer to a question about the extent to which people can be trusted, coded from 1 high trust to 4 low trust;

  2. 2.

    A scale computed from the answers to a range of questions asking about trust in Government, President of country, national parliament, regional government, political parties) coded from 1, high to 4 low trust (see “Appendix”);

  3. 3.

    A scale computed from the answers to a number of questions on trust in formal organisations (courts, police, army, trade unions) coded from 1 high to 4 low trust (see “Appendix”);

  4. 4.

    A scale computed from a number of questions concerning fear of crime (theft from home, harassed or threatened, street robbery) coded from 1 ‘not worried’ to 4 ‘very worried’ (see “Appendix”).

The four indicators together explained 19.5% of the variance, with all of them making a significant contribution. Trust in government had the strongest Beta value and trust in institutions also had a relatively large Beta. Trust in other people and fear of crime made much smaller contributions to the variance explained (see Table 7).

11 Social Integration

Social Integration, in the model describes how people see themselves as living, within this society, the extent to which they feel themselves to be part of that society or of some fraction of it, the extent to which they are not alone but have the support of others. In terms of social integration there is evidence of high levels of personal support from family and friends but lower levels of integration into the wider society (Table 5). For example, over 90 per cent of respondents in the four countries said that they had someone to listen to them, and over 85 per cent in Armenia and Kazakhstan and around 90 per cent in the other two countries that there was someone who would help them in a crisis, but only a tiny proportion, ranging from 1.9 per cent in Georgia to 7.5 per cent in Kyrgyzstan, said they were active members of any organisation. The extent to which people could call upon help from relatives and friends in times of need (social resource) varied between the four countries but was generally lower than for personal support. For example, the number of respondents who could call on support for getting a job varied from less than a third in Georgia to just fewer than 60 per cent in Kyrgyzstan. Overall, respondents in Kyrgyzstan reported the highest levels of support and those in Armenia the lowest, but the differences are not large and the general pattern of social integration is the same across the four countries.

Table 5 Social integration

We selected seven indicators of social integration to represent the various ways individuals can be socially integrated (“Appendix”):

  1. 1.

    Active membership of an organisation coded 0 no, 1 yes;

  2. 2.

    Identifying with the dominant ethnic group, coded 0 no, 1 yes;

  3. 3.

    Married or living with someone as a couple, coded 0 no, 1 yes;

  4. 4.

    Employed, coded 0 not in paid employment, 1 in paid employment;

  5. 5.

    Having a close friend to discuss matters with, coded 1 yes, 2 no;

  6. 6.

    Social Resource Scale computed from the answers to a range of questions about having someone to rely on outside the household (feeling depressed, help in finding a job, need to borrow money) coded 1 yes, 2 no;

  7. 7.

    Personal Support Scale computed from the answers to a range of questions on the availability of personal support(someone to listen, help in a crisis, be yourself with, appreciate you as a person, comfort you when upset) coded 1 yes, 2 no.

The social integration variables in combination explained 9.92 per cent of the variance in general satisfaction. The strongest contribution was made by the social resource scale (a measure of integration with friends and family more than wider groups or the society as a whole), but being an active member of an organisation, identifying with the dominant ethnic group, having a close friend and being married all making a noticeable contribution. Being employed was significant only at the 95 per cent level and the personal support scale did not make a significant contribution (see Table 7).

12 Conditions for Empowerment

Beyond Cohesion and Integration, there is the question of whether people feel that the society in which they live is one in which it is possible to take action, individually or collectively, and take some control over the direction of one’s life, and this is what is meant by Conditions for Empowerment in the Social Quality Module. Levels of perceived personal control are low in these four countries, especially in Armenia (Table 6), with, for example, over two-thirds of Armenians (66.6%), nearly 50 per cent in Kazakhstan (45.1%) and Kyrgyzstan(48.1%)—although less than a quarter (23%) of Georgians—saying that they were unable to enjoy normal daily activities. Over 50 per cent (varying from 51.1% in Armenia to 74.8% in Georgia) said that life was too complicated. Over a third of respondents, rising to over two-thirds in Armenia, said that they felt under constant strain. Only a small minority thought they could influence the national government (9.6% in Armenia, 12.0% in Georgia, 9.3% in Kazakhstan, 23% in Kyrgyzstan) or even take an interest in politics (28.9% in Armenia, 21.5% in Georgia, 34.4% in Kazakhstan, 37.4% in Kyrgyzstan). Over a third of respondents, rising to over 50 per cent in Georgia said they were afraid of illegal arrest. Only around a third of respondents in Armenia (35.2%) and a half in Georgia thought they were free to join any religion they wanted, although the proportions were somewhat higher in Kazakhstan (74.8%) and Kyrgyzstan (62.7%). A majority of respondents, varying from 62 per cent in Georgia to 79 per cent in Kyrgyzstan, said they were free to join any organisation, and over two-thirds said they were free to travel. This suggests that the levels of perceived freedoms are quite high but that levels of perceived control over life and influence on society are much lower.

Table 6 Conditions for empowerment

Health is a resource that enables people to participate in socially valued activities, and lack of good health can be a barrier to participation. Levels of self-reported poor health are comparatively high, especially in Armenia, and noticeably higher for women than men. Psychosocial health was also reported as poor, with women experiencing on average more malaise symptoms than men and with the Armenians having the poorest psychosocial health, followed by the Kyrgyz and the Kazaks, with the Georgians having the best. Education is also a resource that enables people to take more control over their lives, including their health (Mirowsky and Ross 2003a, b). Around forty per cent of respondents in Georgia have had at least some higher education, and around a quarter in the other three countries.

We selected seven indicators of conditions for empowerment:

  1. 1.

    Political influence, a scale computed from answers to questions about the extent of influence on national and regional government coded from 1 high to 4 low;

  2. 2.

    Basic Freedoms Scale computed from the answers to questions on freedom of expression, to travel or to join an organisation, coded from 1 definitely free to 4 definitely not free;

  3. 3.

    The answer to a question on the extent to which the respondent had free choice and control over their life, coded on a five point scale from a great deal of freedom and choice to none at all;

  4. 4.

    Evaluation of health on a four point scale from very good to very bad;

  5. 5.

    A malaise scale measuring a general state of psychological distress (anxiety and depression), a state of misery rather than a symptom of disease (Mirowsky and Ross, 2003b) computed from reporting a number of symptoms—unable to concentrate, loss of confidence, often shaking and trembling, frightening thoughts, spells of exhaustion, feelings of stress—code 1 has the symptom, 2 does not;

  6. 6.

    A personal control scale computed from reporting a number of symptoms (cannot overcome difficulties, unable to enjoy normal day-to-day activities, dissatisfied with work, life is too complicated, impossible to influence things/activities, feeling lonely)—code 1 has the symptom, 2 does not;

  7. 7.

    Education—highest level primary, secondary, tertiary (one indicator of the choices available to an individual).

The variables in total explained 12.9 per cent of the variance in general satisfaction, with political influence and social control making the strongest contributions and freedom and choice, self report health and malaise all making noticeable contributions. Education and political freedom were not significant (Table 7).

Table 7 Influences on satisfaction with life within each quadrant of the social quality model

13 Influences on General Satisfaction

Thus in quadrant-by-quadrant regressions we have shown that each of the quadrants by itself explains a significant amount of the variance between people’s general satisfaction, which we are taking as an indicator of the extent to which the societies in which they live are seen as satisfactory. Economic factors alone account for nearly a quarter of the variance, Social Cohesion for nearly 20 per cent, Social Integration for nearly 10 per cent and Conditions for Social Empowerment around 13 per cent. The quadrants are not independent of each other, however, either theoretically or empirically. Economic factors are to some extent preconditions for social empowerment. Social Cohesion and Social Integration relate to each other in complex ways, and both are affected by economic factors in the sense that there tends to be greater trust in and reliance on personal and family contacts and less on national organisations and structures when the economy is at its worst. It is entirely possible that the effect of some of the quadrants is an artefact—derivative from the effect of others rather than making a contribution which is independent of them—and so we need to check the effect of each on the others’ ability to explain variance in satisfaction. We therefore took all the variables that were significant in the individual quadrant regressions and entered them in a single regression, also controlling for age and gender.

Table 8 (Model 1) shows the results with general satisfaction as the dependent variable. The total variance explained by the full model was 33.3 percent, more than was explained by any of the quadrants alone or any of the variables within them. Economic circumstances continued to make a strong contribution (economic situation and material circumstances), with social cohesion (general trust, trust in government, trust in institutions, fear of crime), social inclusion (social resource, ethnicity and being married) and empowerment (personal control, freedom and control, subjective health, malaise and political influence) all continuing to make significant contribution. Age made a significant contribution, suggesting that older people are less satisfied than younger ones, but the Beta value was low.

Table 8 Factors explaining general satisfaction—overall regression

14 Country Differences in General Satisfaction

Given the differences we noted above in the four countries, we tested the overall model to see if it was a general one that applied to all four countries or if we needed separate models for each country When we added the countries as dummy variables with Kazakhstan as the reference country the amount of variance explained increased for general satisfaction by just under 3 per cent and the three countries entered as dummies all varied significantly from the reference country, suggesting that people in Armenia and Kyrgyzstan are more satisfied than would be predicted by the model and those in Georgia less satisfied (Table 8, Model 2). Given this, we ran the complete model for each country separately (Table 9).

Table 9 Factors explaining general satisfaction by country

The amount of variance explained by the model varied between the countries. The variance explained was 29.7 per cent for Armenia, 29.1 per cent for Kazakhstan, 25.2 per cent for Kyrgyzstan and 38.7 per cent for Georgia. The economic variables made a strong and significant contribution in each country. Economic situation made the largest contribution to the variance explained in Armenia and Kyrgyzstan and was also significant in Kazakhstan. In Georgia and Kazakhstan material circumstances made the largest contribution and also contributed in Kyrgyzstan. Social cohesion also made a significant contribution to the variance explained in all four countries (general trust in people and fear of crime in Kyrgyzstan, trust in government in Armenia, Kazakhstan and Kyrgyzstan and trust in institutions in Georgia). Of the social integration variables, having people to call upon for help in time of need and having a close friend made a significant contribution in Kazakhstan, and being married in Armenia and Kazakhstan. Of the empowerment variables, personal control made a contribution in all the countries, health in Armenia, Kazakhstan and Kyrgyzstan, freedom in Armenia, Georgia and Kazakhstan, malaise in Armenia and political influence in Georgia. Age made a significant contribution in Armenia and Georgia, with younger people being more satisfied, but the Betas were weak and it was significant at only the 95 per cent level. Thus the precise predictors of satisfaction may vary, but variables from each quadrant still have an effect on the prediction, and there is not much variance to be added by the difference between countries once the four quadrants have been taken into account.

15 Conclusions

The main conclusions that can be drawn from the analysis are:

Firstly, levels of satisfaction are low in general in the four countries—the societies are not seen as satisfactory or empowering by their citizens. The levels of satisfaction are comparable with what is found in other CI S countries and, marginally lower than those found in the former communist countries of Central and Eastern Europe. It is surprising that there is not a greater difference given the sharp differences in their trajectories of change during the 1990s and the situation they were in at the turn of the 21st century. The former communist countries that have now become members of the EU had generally transformed into reasonably well-regulated market economies with growing economies, democratic government and civil societies. By contrast the CIS countries had only just begun weak economic recovery, had poorly regulated market economies, authoritarian governments and weak civil societies. Whilst the peoples of the CEE countries had seen the re-establishment of socially integrated societies and could look to a relatively certain future this was not the case for the people living in the countries of Central Asia and the Caucuses. However, as we have noted above, differences in the wording of questions, the scales on which answers are recorded and where questions are asked makes it difficult to compare responses from different surveys and comparisons must be seen as indicative.

Secondly, it is worth noting that the level of general satisfaction was much higher in Kyrgyzstan than in the other three countries, and this was unexpected given it level of poverty. Interestingly, on a normalised index of subjective well-being for young people for 21 CEE and CIS countries Kyrgyzstan had the highest score, +1 SD above the mean—while Kazakhstan was just under −0.5 SD below the mean and Armenia −1.10 with Georgia having the mean score (Richardson et al. 2008). We can only speculate as to why people are more satisfied in Kyrgyzstan than would be expected given its level of economic development. Although it was one of the poorest republics in the FSU it experienced a significant decline in GDP in the 1990s and a growth in inequalities. It has reverted to a pre-modern society with a high proportion of the population dependent on subsistence agriculture and living in rural areas. This means that people spend most of their time in contact with people they know well and are likely to trust. Shevchenko (2009) points out that while levels of trust in general are low in post-Soviet society people trust relatives and friends. Paradoxically, being dependent on subsistence agriculture may also give people more control over their lives. Although the health of the population is not good there was not the dramatic increase in mortality amongst men in mid-life experienced in the other CIS countries and health status is certainly an influence on general well-being. Cultural factors may also be important given that the population is predominantly Sunni-Muslim. Certainly further research into the reasons for the higher levels of subjective satisfaction in Kyrgyzstan is needed.

Thirdly, well-being and satisfaction are influenced by more than economic factors even in societies where economic security is a major concern. The use of a model derived from sociological theory enables us to show that the system and social integration enabling the empowerment of the members of a society is important. What is surprising is the strength of the contribution of variables other than economic circumstances. We should also note that general satisfaction is not influenced by gender, and age only makes a weak contribution—and then not in all the countries. Given that we have derived our indicators from a theoretical model we can compare our findings here with what we found for the wider Europe in 2003 (Abbott and Wallace 2009b, c). The European analysis, like this one, found that all four quadrants of the Model made a significant contribution to the variance explained. It found that the main influences on general satisfaction were; material circumstances, being able to have a decent standard of living as measured by a deprivation scale and being able to make ends meet; social cohesion, trust in other people and trust in government; social integration, feeling left out, and; conditions for empowerment, feeling life is too complicated and health. Being married and frequent contact with friends and relatives also made a significant contribution for the EU 15 but not for the countries of Central and Eastern Europe. The strength of the Betas varied somewhat for the two groups of countries but within the groups the model held. The main differences were that poverty (the inability to make ends meet) and the two trust variables made a larger contribution to the variance explained for the CEE countries and health for the EU15. The findings for the wider EU, like the ones here, suggest that the key explanatory factors are having an adequate income for a decent standard of living, feeling able to trust other people and government, being integrated into society, being in good health and feeling in control of ones life. Economic factors appear to be the most important in both analyses.

Fourthly, in societies that have undergone sudden and dramatic negatively perceived economic and political change, financial security is a major influence on satisfaction, and other variables that influence satisfaction are influenced by material circumstances and may act as buffers that provide some support. Health, for example, is strongly influenced by material circumstances, and economic circumstances condition the extent to which individuals are able to access health care. Reliance on kin and friends for support also provides some buffer to those in poor material circumstances.

Fifthly, the lack of social cohesion in the newly formed societies also clearly has a negative impact on satisfaction with society and life. Lack of trust in other people and in political and civil society institutions has a negative impact on satisfaction as well as being a major barrier to social participation.

Sixthly, those who are empowered and able to exercise control over their lives are more satisfied.

Seventh, while the individual variables explaining the variance in satisfaction vary across the countries, the general model holds for all four countries. This reinforces the importance of using a theoretical model from which to derive the variables for inclusion.

There is evidence that the post-1991 economic and political changes are perceived negatively by a majority of the people in all of the four countries. All four have declined, and not yet recovered, in terms of providing social space in which citizens can take some control of their own lives. Economic hardship is the norm, levels of trust in political and other institutions are low and there is a feeling of lack of control over life in general. This is reflected in the comparatively high levels of dissatisfaction. The analysis indicates that to improve the well-being of the population as measured by their level of satisfaction Governments need not only to develop their economies but also to ensure that they build cohesive societies by ensuring that economic inequalities do not increase and by building trust in political and social institutions. They also need to develop social and economic policies that support social integration and empower people. Important here will be the provision of health and social services as well as opportunities for employment. Economic growth will be important in job creation and generating the resources to improve social and other public services, but the Governments will need to ensure that inequalities are reduced. Building trust will encourage social participation in bridging networks and empower people, as will investment in social services. In other words, Governments need to pay attention to building not only the economy but also the public and social institutions in order to empower their populations. Empowered people who feel in control of their lives will in turn contribute to the building of the economy.

It is evident that the key to satisfaction is a well-regulated society, a society with social and systems integration that is able to deliver a decent standard of living to all. This is common across the EU and CIS countries. The importance of economic factors declines as societies are able to deliver a decent standard of living to the majority of the population. What people want to feel satisfied with their lives is much the same where ever they live. Variation in the factors that explain levels of satisfaction in different societies are related to context and their place within the opportunity structure of the societies in which they live.