Skip to main content
Log in

Strong Bias Towards Analytic Perception in ASD Does not Necessarily Come at the Price of Impaired Integration Skills

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We first demonstrated analytic processing in ASD under conditions in which integral processing seems mandatory in TD observers, a pattern that is often taken to indicate a local default processing in ASD. However, this processing bias does not inevitably come at the price of impaired integration skills. Indeed, examining the same group of individuals with ASD on a task with explicit demands for integrated representations, Experiment 2 showed that the same observers with ASD demonstrated intact spatial integration. The results further showed that performance was not only quantitatively, but also qualitatively comparable to that of TD observers, demonstrating the sensitivity of integration in ASD to the same interactive effects of Gestalt cues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bölte, S., Holtmann, M., Poustka, F., Scheurich, A., & Schmidt, L. (2007). Gestalt perception and local-global processing in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(8), 1493–1504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnel, A., McAdams, S., Smith, B., Berthiaume, C., Bertone, A., Ciocca, V., et al. (2010). Enhanced pure-tone pitch discrimination among persons with autism but not Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2465–2475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnel, A., Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Trudel, M., Gallun, E., & Bonnel, A. M. (2003). Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 226–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouvet, L., Simard-Meilleur, A. A., Paignon, A., Mottron, L., & Donnadieu, S. (2014). Auditory local bias and reduced global interference in autism. Cognition, 131(3), 367–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M. J., Scott, F. J., Fox, S., & Pye, J. (2004). Gestalt processing in autism: Failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 459–469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence: A language-free measure of cognitive ability. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caron, M. J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129(7), 1789–1802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48(3), 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 729–745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evers, K., de-Wit, L., Van der Hallen, R., Haesen, B., Steyaert, J., Noens, I., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Brief report: Reduced grouping interference in children with ASD: Evidence from a multiple object tracking task. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 1–9.

  • Evers, K., Noens, I., Steyaert, J., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Combining strengths and weaknesses in visual perception of children with an autism spectrum disorder: Perceptual matching of facial expressions. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1327–1342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falter, C. M., Grant, K. C., & Davis, G. (2010). Object-based attention benefits reveal selective abnormalities of visual integration in autism. Autism Research, 3(3), 128–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farran, E. K., & Brosnan, M. J. (2011). Perceptual grouping abilities in individuals with autism spectrum disorder; exploring patterns of ability in relation to grouping type and levels of development. Autism Research, 4(4), 283–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, D. J., Hayes, A., & Hess, R. F. (1993). Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field”. Vision Research, 33(2), 173–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foss-Feig, J. H., Tadin, D., Schauder, K. B., & Cascio, C. J. (2013). A substantial and unexpected enhancement of motion perception in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(19), 8243–8249.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426(6967), 664–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Gepner, B., & Mestre, D. R. (2002). Brief report: Postural reactivity to fast visual motion differentiates autistic from children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 231–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadad, B. S. (2012). Sensitivity of spatial integration to perceptual cues is preserved in healthy aging. Vision Research, 60, 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadad, B., Babineau, V., & Burack, J. (2014). Shape perception in autism: The utilization of closure, collinearity and spatial proximity in perceptual organization. Autism Research.

  • Hadad, B. S., & Kimchi, R. (2006). Developmental trends in utilizing perceptual closure for grouping of shape: Effects of spatial proximity and collinearity. Perception and Psychophysics, 68(8), 1264–1273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadad, B. S., & Kimchi, R. (2008). Time course of grouping of shape by perceptual closure: Effects of spatial proximity and collinearity. Perception and Psychophysics, 70(5), 818–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadad, B., Maurer, D., & Lewis, T. L. (2010). The effects of spatial proximity and collinearity on contour integration in adults and children. Vision Research, 50(8), 772–778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. (1996). Studying weak central coherence at low levels: Children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(7), 873–877.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(6), 216–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, N. S., Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Carper, R. A., & Lord, C. (1999). Neuroanatomic contributions to slowed orienting of attention in children with autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 8(1), 61–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, P. (2003). Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(4), 543–551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, P. (2009). Assessing musical skills in autistic children who are not savants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1443–1447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, R. F., & Dakin, S. C. (1997). Absence of contour linking in peripheral vision. Nature, 390(6660), 602–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, R. F., & Dakin, S. C. (1999). Contour integration in the peripheral field. Vision Research, 39(5), 947–959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, R. F., Hayes, A., & Field, D. J. (2003). Contour integration and cortical processing. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 97(2), 105–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci G, Burack JA, Shore DI, Mottron L, Enns JT (2006) Global-local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural versus implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36(1), 117–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 527–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Toddlers with autism spectrum disorder are more successful at visual search than typically developing toddlers. Developmental Science, 14(5), 980–988.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2(3), 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimchi, R., Hadad, B., Behrmann, M., & Palmer, S. E. (2005). Microgenesis and ontogenesis of perceptual organization evidence from global and local processing of hierarchical patterns. Psychological Science, 16(4), 282–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2329–2340.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovács, I. (2000). Human development of perceptual organization. Vision Research, 40(10), 1301–1310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49(Suppl. 2), 467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Ornstein, A. S. (1998). The mean-integral representation of rectangles. Perception and Psychophysics, 60(2), 250–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manjaly, Z. M., Bruning, N., Neufang, S., Stephan, K. E., Brieber, S., Marshall, J. C., et al. (2007). Neurophysiological correlates of relatively enhanced local visual search in autistic adolescents. Neuroimage, 35(1), 283–291.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 255–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., & Szczerbinski, M. (2009). Global and local perceptual style, field-independence, and central coherence: An attempt at concept validation. Advances in Cognitive psychology, 5, 1–26.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, P., Mottron, L., Soulières, I., & Ropar, D. (2010). Susceptibility to the Shepard illusion in participants with autism: Reduced top down influences within perception? Autism Research, 3(3), 113–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1993). A study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain and Cognition, 23(2), 279–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1995). Perspective production in a savant autistic draughtsman. Psychological Medicine, 25(3), 639–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Bouvet, L., Bonnel, A., Samson, F., Burack, J. A., Dawson, M., et al. (2013). Veridical mapping in the development of exceptional autistic abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(2), 209–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131–148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 904–913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hearn, K., Franconeri, S., Wright, C., Minshew, N., & Luna, B. (2013). The development of individuation in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(2), 494–509.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism, 8(3), 229–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(4), 961–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–735.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998a). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 777–783.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998b). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 765–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Saksida, L., Alcántara, J., & Weisblatt, E. (2003). Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak central coherence effects: Experiments in visual configural learning and auditory perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 358(1430), 375–386.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 733–742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R. (1983). Global and local precedence: Selective attention in form and motion perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112(4), 516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Razpurker-Apfeld, I., & Kimchi, R. (2007). The time course of perceptual grouping: The role of segregation and shape formation. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(5), 732–743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A. M., Swettenham, J. G., & Lavie, N. (2012). Lightening the load: Perceptual load impairs visual detection in typical adults but not in autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 544.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(6), 769–778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, C. E., Kravitz, D. J., Freyberg, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Baker, C. I. (2013). Tunnel vision: Sharper gradient of spatial attention in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(16), 6776–6781.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2001). Susceptibility to illusions and performance on visuospatial tasks in individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(4), 539–549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheeren, A. M., & Stauder, J. E. (2008). Broader autism phenotype in parents of autistic children: Reality or myth? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 276–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzkopf, D. S., Anderson, E. J., de Haas, B., White, S. J., & Rees, G. (2014). Larger extrastriate population receptive fields in autism spectrum disorders. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(7), 2713–2724.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–1364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. NeuroReport, 11(12), 2765–2767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (1997). Clusters precede shapes in perceptual organization. Psychological Science, 8(2), 124–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tryon, W. W., & Lewis, C. (2008). An inferential confidence interval method of establishing statistical equivalence that corrects Tryon’s (2001) reduction factor. Psychological Methods, 13(3), 272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wainwright-Sharp, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1993). Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(1), 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(5), 550–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bat-Sheva Hadad.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hadad, BS., Ziv, Y. Strong Bias Towards Analytic Perception in ASD Does not Necessarily Come at the Price of Impaired Integration Skills. J Autism Dev Disord 45, 1499–1512 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2293-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2293-5

Keywords

Navigation