Skip to main content
Log in

Relations of Growth in Effortful Control to Family Income, Cumulative Risk, and Adjustment in Preschool-age Children

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study examined growth in effortful control (executive control, delay ability) in relation to income, cumulative risk (aggregate of demographic and psychosocial risk factors), and adjustment in 306 preschool-age children (50 % girls, 50 % boys) from families representing a range of income (29 % at- or near-poverty; 28 % lower-income; 25 % middle-income; 18 % upper-income), with 4 assessments starting at 36–40 month. Income was directly related to levels of executive control and delay ability. Cumulative risk accounted for the effects of income on delay ability but not executive control. Higher initial executive control and slope of executive control and delay ability predicted academic readiness, whereas levels, but not growth, of executive control and delay ability predicted social competence and adjustment problems. Low income is a marker for lower effortful control, which demonstrates additive or mediating effects in the relation of income to children’s preschool adjustment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, B., Brown, E., & Izard, C. (2004). The relations between contextual risk, earned income and the school adjustment of children from economically disadvantaged families. Developmental Psychology, 40, 204–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allan, N., & Lonigan, C. (2011). Examining the dimensionality of effortful control in preschool children and its relation to academic and socioemotional indicators. Developmental Psychology, 47, 905–915.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchaine, T. P., Hinshaw, S. P., & Pang, K. L. (2010). Comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and early-onset conduct disorder: biological, environmental, and developmental mechanisms. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 17, 327–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2008). Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 821–843.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2011). Development of inhibitory control among prenatally cocaine exposed and non-cocaine exposed youths from late childhood to early adolescence: The effects of gender and risk and subsequent aggressive behavior. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 33, 47–60.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., & Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and Differentiating Aspects of Self-Regulation: Effortful Control, Executive Functioning, and Links to Negative Affectivity. Emotion, 13, 47–63. doi:10.1037/a0029536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, L., Rimm-Kaufman, S., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. (2009). The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children's academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 337–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, J., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. (2003). Characteristics of resilient youths living in poverty: The role of self-regulatory processes. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 139–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, J., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. (2009). Self-regulation and its relations to adaptive functioning in low income youths. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79, 19–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, L., Schafer, J., & Kam, C. (2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, 330–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, E. P., Bruce, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2002). The anterior attention network: Associations with temperament and neuroendocrine activity in 6-year-old children. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 43–56. doi:10.1002/dev.10012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • deBoo, G., & Kolk, A. (2007). Ethnic and gender differences in temperament, and the relationship between temperament and Depressive and Aggressive mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1756–1766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. (2010). Uncovering the neural basis of resisting immediate gratification while pursuing long-term goals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 6178–6179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Kalil, A. (2010). Early-childhood poverty and adult attainment, behavior and health. Child Development, 81, 306–325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Reiser, M., Murphy, B. C., Losoya, S., & Guthrie, I. (2001a). The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development, 72, 1112–1134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Gershoff, E., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Cumberland, A., Losoya, S., et al. (2001b). Mother’s emotional expressivity and children’s behavior problems and social competence: Mediation through children’s regulation. Developmental Psychology, 37, 475–490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Fabes, R., Smith, C., Reiser, M., Shepard, S., Losoya, A., Guthrie, I., Murphy, B., & Cumberland, A. (2003). The relations of effortful control and ego control to children's resiliency and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 761–776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. (2003). A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 924–933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., & Cassells, R. C. (2014). Childhood poverty, cumulative risk exposure, and mental health in emerging adults. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 287–296. doi:10.1177/2167702613501496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73, 1238–1248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2007). Childhood poverty and health: Cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychological Science, 18, 953–957. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02008.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty and young adults’ allostatic load: The mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychological Science, 23, 979–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., & Wachs, T. D. (2010). Chaos and its influence on children’s development: An ecological perspective. (G. W. Evans & T. D. Wachs, Eds.). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W., Li, D., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1342–1396. doi:10.1037/a0031808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstadt, C., Hong, Y., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3.5-7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gresham, F. M., & Elliot, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K., Lee, W., & Zelazo, P. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, J., Lavigne, J. V., Gouze, K. R., LeBailly, S. A., & Bryant, F. B. (2013). Multi-domain models of risk factors for depression and anxiety symptoms in preschoolers: Evidence for common and specific factors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 705–722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2009). Independence and interplay between maternal and child risk factors for preschool problem behaviors? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 312–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A. (2010). Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 20–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J., & Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in “hot” and “cool” tasks differentially predicts children’s behavior problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 43–56.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, K., Lengua, L., & Monahan, K. (2013). Differentiating executive and motivational components of self-regulation: Differences in trajectories, predictors and adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 57–69. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9665-0.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T., Koenig, A., & Vandegeest, K. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. Manual: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavigne, J. V., Gouze, K. R., Hopkins, J., Bryant, F. B., & LeBailly, S. A. (2012). A multi-domain model of risk factors for ODD symptoms in a community sample of 4-year olds. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 741–757.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L. (2003). Associations among emotionality, self-regulation, adjustment problems and positive adjustment in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 595–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L. (2006). Growth in temperament and parenting as predictors of adjustment during children’s transition to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 42, 819–832.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L., Honorado, E., & Bush, N. (2007). Cumulative risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 40–55.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, L. J., Zalewski, M., Fisher, P., Moran, L. (in press). Does HPA-Axis dysregulation account for the effects of income on effortful control and adjustment in preschool? Infant and Child Development.

  • Li-Grining, C. (2007). Effortful control among low-income preschoolers in three cities: Stability, change, and individual differences. Developmental Psychology, 43, 208–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linver, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Kohen, D. (2002). Family processes as pathways from income to young children’s development. Developmental Psychology, 38, 719–734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. H. (1995). A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 30, 41–62.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, M., Cameron, C., Connor, C., Farris, C., Jewkes, A., & Morrison, F. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43, 947–959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 1373–1386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mistry, R., Vandewater, E., Huston, A., & McLoyd, V. (2002). Economic well-being and children’s social adjustment: The role of family process in an ethnically diverse low-income sample. Child Development, 73, 935–951.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mistry, R., Benner, A., Biesanz, J., Clark, S., & Howes, C. (2010). Family and social risk, and parental investments during the early childhood years as predictors of low-income children’s school readiness outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 432–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., van der Pennen, E., Sigmond, R., & Mayer, B. (2008). Symptoms of anxiety, depression, and aggression in non-clinical children: Relationships with self-report and performance-based measures of attention and effortful control. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 39, 455–467.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthen, L., & Muthen, B. (2010). Mplus User’s Guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obradovic, J., Bush, N., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N., & Boyce, W. (2010). Biological sensitivity to context: The interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socio-emotional behaviour and school readiness. Child Development, 81, 270–289.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ponitz, C., McClelland, M., Jewkes, A., Connor, C., Farris, C., & Morrison, F. (2008). Touch your toes! Developing a direct measure of behavioral regulation in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 141–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C. C., Blackburn, E. K., Bancroft, M., & Torp, N. (1999). Relations between effective emotional self-regulation, attentional control, and low-income preschoolers’ social competence with peers. Early Education and Development, 10, 333–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C., Jones, S., Li Grining, C., Zhai, F., Bub, K., & Pressler, E. (2011). CRSP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development, 82, 362–378.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C. C., Blair, C. W., & Willoughby, M. (2013). Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds’ executive function: New Perspectives on models of differential susceptibility. Developmental Psychology, 49, 292–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razza, R. A., Martin, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2010). Associations among family environment, sustained attention, and school readiness for low-income children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1528–1542.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, M., Pien, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 131–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., Lanza, S. T., & Blair, C. (2011). Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 638–662.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M., & Bates, J. (2006). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg, N. Damon, & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th Ed.) (pp. 99–166). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M., Ahadi, S., & Evans, D. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sandler, I., Ramirez, R., & Reynolds, K. (1986, August). Life stress for children of divorce, bereavedc and asthmatic children. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association. D.C.: Washington.

  • Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of Children: Cognitive Applications, 4th Edition. San Diego, CA: Sattler Publishing, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sektnan, M., McClelland, M., Acock, A., & Morrison, F. (2010). Relations between early family risk, children's behavioral regulation, and academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 464–479.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheese, B. E., Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., White, L. K., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2008). Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy. Infant Behavior & Development, 31, 501–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sulik, M. J., Huera, S., Zerr, A. A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., DiGiunta, L., Pina, A. A., Eggum, N. D., Sallquist, J., Edwards, A., Kupfer, A., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Wilson, S. B., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Taylor, H. B. (2010). The Factor Structure of Effortful Control and Measurement Invariance Across Ethnicity and Sex in a High-Risk Sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 8–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, J., Valiente, C., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2012). Predicting academic achievement from cumulative home risk: The mediating roles of effortful control, academic relationships, and school avoidance. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 58, 375–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valiente, C., Lemery-Calfant, K., & Castro, K. (2007). Children’s effortful control and academic competence: Mediation through school liking. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon-Feagans, L., & Cox, M. (2013). The family life project: And epidemiological and developmental study of young children living in poor rural communities: I. Poverty, rurality, parenting and risk: An introduction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78, 1–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Tominey, S. L., & Acock, A. C. (2010). The influence of demographic risk factors on children’s behavioral regulation in prekindergarten and kindergarten. Early Education and Development, 22, 461–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, N. M., Evans, G. W., Beavis, A., & Ong, A. D. (2010). Early childhood poverty, cumulative risk exposure, and body mass index trajectories through young adulthood. American Journal of Public Health, 100, 2507–2512.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J., Clark, C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Kiff, C. J., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Understanding the relation of low income to HPA-axis functioning in preschool children: Cumulative family risk and parenting as pathways to disruptions in cortisol. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43, 924–942. doi:10.1007/s10578-012-0304-3.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, Q., Chen, S., & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 112–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Support for this research was provided by NICHD grant R01HD054465 awarded to the first author. The authors thank the families who participated in this study.

Conflict of Interest

There are no conflicts of interests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Liliana J. Lengua.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lengua, L.J., Moran, L., Zalewski, M. et al. Relations of Growth in Effortful Control to Family Income, Cumulative Risk, and Adjustment in Preschool-age Children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 43, 705–720 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9941-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9941-2

Keywords

Navigation