Skip to main content
Log in

Effortful Control in “Hot” and “Cool” Tasks Differentially Predicts Children’s Behavior Problems and Academic Performance

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Effortful control (EC), the capacity to deliberately suppress a dominant response and perform a subdominant response, rapidly developing in toddler and preschool age, has been shown to be a robust predictor of children’s adjustment. Not settled, however, is whether a view of EC as a heterogeneous rather than unidimensional construct may offer advantages in the context of predicting diverse developmental outcomes. This study focused on the potential distinction between “hot” EC function (delay-of-gratification tasks that called for suppressing an emotionally charged response) and more abstract “cool” EC functions (motor inhibition tasks, suppressing-initiating response or Go-No Go tasks, and effortful attention or Stroop-like tasks). Children (N = 100) were observed performing EC tasks at 38 and 52 months. Mothers, fathers, and teachers rated children’s behavior problems and academic performance at 67, 80, and 100 months, and children participated in a clinical interview at 100 months. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analyses with latent variables produced consistent findings across all informants: Children’s scores in “hot” EC tasks, presumably engaging emotion regulation skills, predicted behavior problems but not academic performance, whereas their scores in “cool” EC tasks, specifically those engaging effortful attention, predicted academic performance but not behavior problems. The models of EC as a heterogeneous construct offered some advantages over the unidimensional models. Methodological and clinical implications of the findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allan, N. P., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Examining the dimensionality of effortful control in preschool children and its relation to academic and socioemotional indicators. Developmental Psychology, 47, 905–915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). (DSM-IV-TR) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.. text revision.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arseneault, L., Kim-Cohen, J., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2005). Psychometric evaluation of 5-and 7-year-old children’s self-reports of conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 537–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, W. T., Essex, M. J., Woodward, H. R., Measelle, J. R., Ablow, J. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. I: exploring the “headwaters” of early life morbidities. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 580–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breton, J. J., Bergeron, L., Valla, J. P., Berthiaume, C., & Gaudet, N. (1999). Quebec Child Mental Health Survey: prevalence of DSM-III-R mental health disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 375–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 337–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulated cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4–6, 215–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: a multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 477–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 300–365). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P. M., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2009). Emotion regulation, risk, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1327–1330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Smith, K. D. (1994). Expressive control during a disappointment: variations related to preschoolers’ behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 30, 835–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P. M., Luby, J., & Sullivan, M. W. (2008). Emotions and the development of childhood depression: bridging the gap. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 141–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A., Warren-Khot, H. K., Bassett, H. H., Wyatt, T., & Perna, A. (2012). Factor structure of self-regulation in preschoolers: testing models of a field-based assessment for predicting early school readiness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 386–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9(04), 633–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 25–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., Shepard, S. A., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Cumberland, A. J. (2003). The relations of EC and ego-control to children’s resiliency and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 761–776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Champion, C., & Ma, Y. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: an emerging construct. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 236–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., Valiente, C., Losoya, S. H., Guthrie, I. K., & Thompson, M. (2004). The relations of EC and impulsivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child Development, 75(1), 25–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A. C., & Moehr, M. J. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 33–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., & Kupfer, D. J. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 588–603.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 55–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (2002). Child Symptom Inventory-4: Screening and norms manual. Stony Brook: Checkmate Plus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gadow, K. D., Sprafkin, J., & Nolan, E. E. (2001). DSM-IV Symptoms in community and clinic preschool children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1383–1392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gusdorf, L. M. A., Karreman, A., van Aken, M. A. G., Dekovic, M., & van Tuijl, C. (2011). The structure of effortful control in preschoolers and its relation to externalizing problems. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 612–634.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huijbregts, S. C. J., Warren, A. J., de Sonneville, L. M. J., & Swaab-Barneveld, H. (2008). Hot and cool forms of inhibitory control and externalizing behavior in children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: an exploratory study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(3), 323–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 418–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, K., Hipwell, A. E., Hinze, A. E., & Babinski, D. E. (2009). The association of pain and depression in preadolescent girls: moderation by race and pubertal stage. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 727–737.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). EC as a personality characteristic of young children: antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: from toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68, 263–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Jimenez, N. B., Hollatz, A. L., & Woodard, J. (2009). Guilt and effortful control: two mechanisms that prevent disruptive developmental trajectories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 322–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: a developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., White, J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Delay of gratification, psychopathology, and personality: is low self-control specific to externalizing problems? Journal of Personality, 64, 107–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: a cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 677–692.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., & Hocevar, D. (1988). A new, more powerful approach to multitrait-multimethod analyses: application of second-order confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(1), 107–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 271–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Temperament and self- regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self- regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 357–370). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rueda, C. (In press). Effortful control. In M. Zentner & R. Shiner (Eds.), The Handbook of Temperament. New York: Guilford Press.

  • Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Dalen, L., Daley, D., & Remington, B. (2002). Are planning, working memory, and inhibition associated with individual differences in preschool ADHD symptoms? Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 255–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sprafkin, J., Gadow, K. D., Salisbury, H., Schneider, J., & Loney, J. (2002). Further evidence of reliability and validity of the Child Symptom Inventory-4: parent checklist in clinically referred boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 513–524.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically-Based Tests for the Number of Common Factors. Paper presented at the annual Spring meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.

  • Sulik, M. J., Huerta, S., Zerr, A. A., Eisenberg, A., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Di Giunta, L., Pina, A. A., Eggum, N. D., Sallquist, D., Edwards, A., Kupfer, A., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Wilson, S. B., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Taylor, H. B. (2010). The factor structure of effortful control and measurement invariance across ethnicity and sex in a high-risk sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 8–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. A., Flood, M. F., & Lundquist, L. (1995). Emotional regulation and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (Emotion, cognition, and representation, Vol. 6, pp. 261–299). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valla, J. P. (2000). The Dominic Interactive: Instruction Manual. Montreal, QC: D.I.M.A.T. Inc.

  • Valla, J. P., Bergeron, L., & Smolla, N. (2000). The Dominic-R: a pictorial interview for 6- to 11-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research has been funded by the grants from NIMH, R01 MH63096 and K02 MH01446, and R01 HD069171 from NICHD to Grazyna Kochanska, who was also supported by the Stuit Professorship. We thank many individuals, including Jarilyn Akabogu, Jessica O’Bleness, and Robin Barry for help with data collection, coding, and file creation, and the parents and children in Family Study for their enthusiastic commitment to this research. The HBQ was made available free of charge by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Psychopathology and Development (David J. Kupfer, Network Chair).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sanghag Kim.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kim, S., Nordling, J.K., Yoon, J.E. et al. Effortful Control in “Hot” and “Cool” Tasks Differentially Predicts Children’s Behavior Problems and Academic Performance. J Abnorm Child Psychol 41, 43–56 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4

Keywords

Navigation