Skip to main content
Log in

Individual Priming in Virtual Team Decision-Making

  • Published:
Group Decision and Negotiation Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Virtual teams have different interactions than face-to-face teams because they rely on information and communication technologies, which can impede or assist certain human cognitive processes. Past research has shown that although virtual teams exchange more information than face-to-face teams, poor decisions often result, because team members do not consider the unique information they receive from others. Drawing from cognitive psychology, our research explored a unique way to improve team decision-making through the use of cognitive priming. We proposed that priming group members to pay attention to others or to engage in counterfactual thinking would improve team members’ cognition and, therefore, team performance. Prior research with individuals and brainstorming teams has shown these forms of priming to improve performance; however, no research has attempted to use priming to improve the outcomes of virtual team decision-making, which requires deeper interaction and cognitive involvement than brainstorming. We performed two lab experiments using primes that have been found to improve the individual decision-making process. We found that priming had some impact, but it did not significantly improve decision quality. Various reasons are discussed to explain why priming techniques may not be as powerful in teams as in individuals, and future research ideas are suggested to build on our initial work on priming in virtual team decision-making.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allchin D (1999) Negative results as positive knowledge, and zeroing in on significant problems. Mar Ecol 793 Prog Ser 191:303–305 http://www.int-res.com/articles/theme/m191p301.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antunes P, Ferreira A (2011) Developing collaboration awareness support from a cognitive perspective. In: Proceedings of the 44th annual hawaii international conference on system sciences, January 4–7, 2011, Computer Society Press, pp 1–10

  • Ask K, Granhag PA (2005) Motivational sources of confirmation bias in criminal investigations: the need for cognitive closure. J Investig Psychol Offender Profiling 2(1):43–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Chartrand TL (2000) A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In: Reis H, Judd C (eds) Handbook of research methods in social psychology. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 253–285

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Lee-Chai A, Barndollar K, Trötschel R (2001) The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. J Pers Soc Psychol 81(6):1014–1027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann N, Kuhl J (2005) Positive affect and flexibility: overcoming the precedence of global over local processing of visual information. Motiv Emot 29(2):123–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell BS, Kozlowski SWJ (2002) A typology of virtual teams: implications for effective leadership. Group Organ Manag 27(1):14–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair IV, Banaji MR (1996) Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. J Pers Soc Psychol 70(6):1142–1163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1999) Affective norms for english words (ANEW): instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida

  • Briggs RO, de Vreede GJ, Nunamaker JF (2004) Collaboration engineering with thinklets to pursue sustained success with group support systems. J Manag Inf Syst 19(4):31–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Browman HI (1999) Negative results. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 191: 301–302. http://www.int-res.com/articles/theme/m191p301.pdf

  • Burnham T, McCabe K, Smith VL (2000) Friend-or-foe intentionality priming in an extensive form trust game. J Econ Behav Organ 43(1):57–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne RMJ, Girotto V (2009) Cognitive processes in counterfactual thinking. In: Markman KD, Klein WMP, Surh JA (eds) Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. Psychology Press, New York, pp 151–160

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalfoun P, Frasson C (2011) Subliminal cues while teaching: HCI technique for enhanced learning. Adv Hum-Comput Interact 2011:1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chudoba KM, Wynn E, Lu M, Watson-Manheim MB (2005) How virtual are we? measuring virtuality and understanding its impact in a global organization. Inf Syst J 15(4):279–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen SG, Bailey DE (1997) What makes teams work: group effectiveness research from the shop floor to the executive suite. J Manag 23(3):239–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly T, Reb J (2003) Omission bias in vaccination decisions: where’s the ’omission’? Where’s the ’bias’? Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 91(2):186–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly T, Zeelenberg M (2002) Regret in decision making. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11(6):212–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Credé M, Sniezek JA (2003) Group judgment processes and outcomes in video-conferencing versus face-to-face groups. Int J Hum-Comput Stud 59(6):875–897

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curseu PL, Schruijer S (2008) The effects of framing on inter-group negotiation. Group Decis Negot 17(4):347–362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean JW Jr, Sharfman MP (1993) Procedural rationality in the strategic decision making process. J Manag Stud 30(4):587–610

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean JW Jr, Sharfman MP (1996) Does decision process matter? A study of strategic decision-making effectiveness. Acad Manag J 39(2):368–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeCoster J, Claypool HM (2004) A meta-analysis of priming effects on impression formation supporting a general model of information biases. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 8(1):2–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis AR (1996) Information exchange and use in group decision making: you can lead a group to information, but you can’t make it think. MIS Q 20(4):433–455

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis AR, Hilmer KM, Taylor NJ (1997–1998) Information exchange and use in GSS and verbal group decision making: effect of minority influence. J Manag Inf Syst 14(3):61–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis AR, Minas RK, Bhagwatwar A (2012) Sparking creativity: improving electronic brainstorming with individual cognitive priming. In: Proceedings of the 45th annual Hawaii international conference on system sciences, January 4–7, 2012, Computer Society Press, pp 139–148

  • Dennis AR, Wixom BH, Vandenberg RJ (2001) Understanding fit and appropriation effects in group support systems via meta-analysis. MIS Q 25(2):167–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimoka A, Banker RD, Benbasat I, Davis FD, Dennis AR, Gefen D, Gupta A, Ischebeck A, Kenning P, Pavlou PA, Müller-Putz G, Riedl R, vom Brocke J, Weber B (2012) On the use of neurophysiological tools in information systems research: Developing a research agenda for NeuroIS. MIS Q, forthcoming

  • Duncker K (1945) On problem solving. Psychological monographs. Am Psychol Assoc 58(5)

  • Espinosa JA, Armour F, Boh WF (2011) The role of group cognition in enterprise architecting. Proceedings of the 44th annual Hawaii international conference on system sciences, January 4–7, 2011, Computer Society Press, pp 1–10

  • Festinger L (1957) A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky AD, Moskowitz GB (2000) Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alternatives. J Exp Soc Psychol 36(4):384–409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert DT, Morewedge CK, Risen JL, Wilson TD (2004) Looking forward to looking backward: the misprediction of regret. Assoc Psychol Sci 15(5):346–350

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glass GV (1966) Testing homogeneity of variances. Am Educ Res J 3(3):187–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf P, Mandler G, Haden P (1982) Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects. Science 218(4578):1243–1244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf P, Schacter DL (1985) Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 11(3):501–518

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heninger WG, Dennis AR, Hilmer KM (2006) Individual cognition and dual-task interference in group support systems. Inf Syst Res 17(4):415–424

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins ET, Chaires WM (1980) Accessibility of interrelational constructs: implications for stimulus encoding and creativity. J Exp Soc Psychol 16(4):348–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilmer KM, Dennis AR (2001) Stimulating thinking in decision making: cultivating better decisions with groupware one individual at a time. J Manag Inf Syst 17(3):93–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby LL, Dallas M (1981) On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. J Exp Psychol Gen 110(3):306–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonas E, Schulz-Hardt S, Frey D, Thelen N (2001) Confirmation bias in sequential information search after preliminary decisions:an expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective exposure to information. J Pers Soc Psychol 80(4):557–571

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahai SS, Carroll E, Jestice R (2007) Team collaboration in virtual worlds. SIGMIS Database 38(4):61–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Miller D (1986) Norm theory: comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychol Rev 93(2):136–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay AC, Wheeler SC, Bargh JA, Ross L (2004) Material priming: the influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 95(1):83–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr DS, Murthy US (2004) Divergent and convergent idea generation in teams: a comparison of computer-mediated and face-to-face communication. Group Decis Negot 13(4):381–399

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kersten GE, Cray D (1996) Perspectives on representation and analysis of negotiation: towards cognitive support systems. Group Decis Negot 5(4–6):433–467

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolfschoten GL (2011) Cognitive load in collaboration—brainstorming. In: Proceedings of the 44th annual Hawaii international conference on system sciences, January 4–7, 2011, Computer Society Press, pp 1–9

  • Kolfschoten GL, Lee C (2010) Self-guiding group support systems: can groups use GSS without support? In: Proceedings of the 43rd annual Hawaii international conference on system sciences, January 5–8, 2010, Computer Society Press, pp 1–6

  • Kolfschoten GL, Vreede G-J, Pietron LR (2011) A training approach for the transition of repeatable collaboration processes to practitioners. Group Decis Negot 20(3):347–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat A, Feuerstein N (1976) The recovery of incidentally acquired information. Acta Psychol 40(6):463–464

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen RJ, Ketelaar T (1991) Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. J Pers Soc Psychol 61(1):132–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin A, Chao LL (2001) Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11(2):194–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martz WB, Shepherd MM (2004) Group consensus: the impact of multiple dialogues. Group Decis Negot 13(4):315–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrath JE (1991) Time, interaction, and performance (TIP): a theory of groups. Small Group Res 22(2):147–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyers-Levy J, Zhu R (2007) The influence of ceiling height: the effect of priming on the type of processing that people use. J Consum Res 34(2):174–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montoya-Weiss MM, Massey AP, Song M (2001) Getting it together: temporal coordination and conflict management in global virtual teams. Acad Manag J 44(6):1251–1262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagasundaram M, Dennis AR (1993) When a group is not a group. Small Group Res 24(4):463–489

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Leary D (2011) The emergence of individual knowledge in a group setting: mitigating cognitive fallacies. Group Decis Negot 20(1):3–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otten S, Wentura D (1999) About the impact of automaticity in the minimal group paradigm: evidence from affective priming tasks. J Soc Psychol 29(8):1049–1071

    Google Scholar 

  • Pendry L, Carrick R (2001) Doing what the mob do: priming effects on conformity. Eur J Soc Psychol 31(1):83–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty RE, DeMarree KG, Briñol P, Horcajo J, Strathman AJ (2008) Need for cognition can magnify or attenuate priming effects in social judgment. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 34(7):900–912

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pichon I, Boccato G, Saroglou V (2007) Nonconscious influences of religion on prosociality: a priming study. Eur J Soc Psychol 37(5):1032–1045

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell CA (1994) Cognitive hurdles in the use of decision support systems to enhance problem understanding. Group Decis Negot 3(4):413–421

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reb J (2008) Regret aversion and decision process quality: effects of regret salience on decision process carefulness. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 105(2):169–182

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reb J, Connolly T (2009) Myopic regret avoidance: feedback avoidance and learning in repeated decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 109(2):182–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinig B (2003) Toward an understanding of satisfaction with the process and outcomes of teamwork. J Manag Inf Syst 19(4):65–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinig B, Briggs RO, Shepherd MM, Yen J, Nunamaker JF, Jr (1995–1996) Affective reward and the adoption of group support systems: productivity is not always enough. J Manag Inf Syst 12(3):171–185

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinig B, Shin B (2002) The dynamic effects of group support systems on group meetings. J Manag Inf Syst 19(2):303–325

    Google Scholar 

  • Robert L, Dennis AR (2005) The paradox of richness: a cognitive model of media choice. IEEE Trans Prof Commun 48(1):10–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson MD, Moeller SK, Ode S (2010) Extraversion and reward-related processing: incentive motivation in affective priming tasks. Emotion 10(5):615–626

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santanen EL, Briggs RO, de Vreede GJ (2004) Causal relationships in creative problem solving: compaing faciliatation interventions for ideation. J Manag Inf Syst 20(4):167–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter DL (1987) Implicit memory: history and current status. J Exp Psychol Mem Learn Cognit 13(3):501–518

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt JB, Montoya-Weiss MM, Massey AP (2001) New product development decision-making effectiveness: comparing individuals, face-to-face teams, and virtual teams. Decis Sci 32(4):575–600

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shantz A, Latham GP (2009) An exploratory field experiment of the effect of subconscious and conscious goals on employee performance. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 109(1):9–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd MM, Briggs RO, Reinig BA, Yen J, Nunamaker JR (1995–1996) Invoking social comparison to improve electronic brainstorming: Beyond anonymity. J Manag Inf Syst 12(3): 155–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonson I (1992) The influence of anticipating regret and responsibility on purchase decisions. J Consum Res 19(1):105–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srull TK, Wyer RS Jr (1979) The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: some determinants and implications. J Pers Soc Psychol 37(10):1660–1672

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl G (2006) Analyzing and designing the group cognition experience. Int J Co-op Inf Syst 15(2):157–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stajkovic AD, Locke EA, Blair ES (2006) A first examination of the relationships between primed subconscious goals, assigned conscious goals, and task performance. J Appl Psychol 91(5):1172–1180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stasser G (1992) Information salience and the discovery of hidden profiles by decision-making groups: a thought experiment. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 52(1):156–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiner ID (1972) Group process and productivity. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Strahan EJ, Spencer SJ, Zanna MP (2002) Subliminal priming and persuasion: striking while the iron is hot. J Exp Soc Psychol 38(6):556–568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor SE (1991) Asymmetric effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychol Bull 110(1):67–85

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E, Schacter DL, Stark HA (1982) Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 8(4):336–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster DM, Richter L, Kruglanski AW (1996) On leaping to conclusions when feeling tired: mental fatigue effects on impressional primacy. J Exp Soc Psychol 32(2):181–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitworth B, Gallupe B, McQueen R (2000) A cognitive three-process model of computer-mediated group interaction. Group Decis Negot 9(5):431–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woolley AW, Chabris CF, Pentland A, Hashmi N, Malone TW (2010) Evidence for a collective intelligence factor in the performance of human groups, Science 330 (29 October):686–688

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong EM, Galinsky AD, Kray LJ (2009) The counterfactual mind-set: a decade of research. In: Markman KD, Klein WMP, Surh JA (eds) Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. Psychology Press, New York, pp 161–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Wroe AL, Turner N, Salkovskis PM (2004) Understanding and predicting parental decisions about early childhood immunizations. Health Psychol 23(1):33–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M (1999) The use of crying over spilled milk: a note on the rationality and functionality of regret. Philos Psycol 12(3):325–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, Beattie J (1997) Consequences of regret aversion 2: additional evidence for effects of feedback on decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 72(1):63–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, Beattie J, van der Pligt J, de Vries NK (1996) Consequences of regret aversion: effects of expected feedback on risky decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 65(2):148–158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, Pieters R (2007) A theory of regret regulation 1.0. J Consum Psychol 17(1):3–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg M, van Dijk WW, Manstead ASR, van der Pligt J (1998) The experience of regret and disappointment. Cognit Emot 12(2):221–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Rosh Dhanawade for his assistance in generating potential attention priming words and collecting data during Study 1.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jordan B. Barlow.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bartelt, V.L., Dennis, A.R., Yuan, L. et al. Individual Priming in Virtual Team Decision-Making. Group Decis Negot 22, 873–896 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10726-012-9333-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10726-012-9333-3

Keywords

Navigation