Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

To add or to multiply in open problems? Unraveling children’s relational preference using a mixed-method approach

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research demonstrated that some children inappropriately solve multiplicative missing-value word problems additively, while others inappropriately solve additive missing-value word problems multiplicatively. Besides lacking skills, children’s preference for additive or multiplicative relations has been shown to contribute to those errors. The present research, using a mixed-method approach, investigated the nature of upper primary school children’s relational preference by empirically examining characteristics of intuitions that had been postulated previously. After administering a pre-test, selected children who preferred additive or multiplicative relations further participated in one of two studies using open problems for which both types of relations were appropriate: either a reaction time study (n = 110) in which children’s acceptance behavior and reaction times were measured or a semi-structured individual interview study (n = 18) in which their answers, verbalizations, and conviction scores were collected. Results of both studies revealed that relational preference was perseverant and exerted a coercive effect on children’s reasoning: Children mostly considered only the preferential type of relation as an appropriate answer in open problems and rejected alternative answers. Furthermore, relational preference appeared as immediate, self-evident, and certain: Children rejected the non-preferential answer more quickly than an irrelevant distractor of comparable size, experienced difficulties in justifying why they gave their preferential answer, and were very convinced of this preferential answer. While this characterization held for both relational preferences, it was especially prominent for the multiplicative one. These results not only have implications for research on and educational practice in multiplicative and additive reasoning but also for the measurement of relational preference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. At the end of the pre-test and the interview, some items containing non-integer ratios were also included, but these are not considered in the present paper. We focus on the items that are similar to these administered in study 1.

References

  • Babai, R., Levyadun, T., Stavy, R., & Tirosh, D. (2006). Intuitive rules in science and mathematics: A reaction time study. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 37, 913–924. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207390600794958

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, D. H., Littlefield, A., & Geary, D. C. (2012). The codevelopment of skill at and preference for use of retrieval-based processes for solving addition problems: Individual and sex differences from first to sixth graders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 78–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behr, M., & Harel, G. (1990). Understanding the multiplicative structure. In G. Booker, P. Cobb, & T. N. de Merldicutti (Eds.), Proceedings of 14th conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 27–34). Mexico, Mexico: Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Technologia, Gobierno del Estado de Morelos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, T. W., & Levine, S.C. (2012). Child proportional scaling: Is 1/3 = 2/6 = 3/9 = 4/12? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 516–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.001

  • Boyer, T. W., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Development of proportional reasoning: Where young children go wrong. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1478–1490. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013110

  • Clark, F. B., & Kamii, C. (1996). Identification of multiplicative thinking in children in grades 1-5. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 41–51. https://doi.org/10.2307/749196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, K., & Post, T. (1993). Connecting research to teaching proportional reasoning. Mathematics Teacher, 86(5), 404–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, K., Post, T., & Currier, S. (1993). Learning and teaching ratio and proportion: Research implications. In D. T. Owens (Ed.), Research ideas for the classroom: Middle grades mathematics (pp. 159–178). New York, NY: Macmillan Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1984). Essentials of psychological testing (4th ed.). New York, NY: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csíkos, C. (2016). Strategies and performance in elementary students’ three-digit mental addition. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 91, 123–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-015-9658-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Bock, D., Van Dooren, W., Janssens, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2002). Improper use of linear reasoning: An in-depth study of the nature and the irresistibility of secondary school students’ errors. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 50, 311–334. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021205413749

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Degrande, T., Verschaffel, L., & Van Dooren, W. (2018). Beyond additive and multiplicative reasoning abilities: How preference enters the picture. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 33, 559–576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-017-0352-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Degrande, T., Verschaffel, L., & Van Dooren, W. (2019). To add or to multiply? An investigation of the role of preference in children’s solutions of word problems. Learning and Instruction, 61, 60–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.01.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, C., Llinares, S., Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2012). The development of students’ use of additive and proportional methods along primary and secondary school. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 27, 421–438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-011-0087-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics: An educational approach. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Reidel Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1999). Intuitions and schemata in mathematical reasoning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 38, 11–50. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1003488222875

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, S. (2015). When to add and when to multiply: A textbook analysis on task characteristics in additive and proportional reasoning [Unpublished master’s thesis]. KU Leuven, Belgium.

  • Gillard, E., Van Dooren, W., Schaeken, W., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Proportional reasoning as a heuristic-based process: Time pressure and dual-task considerations. Experimental Psychology, 56, 92–99. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.2.92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, G., & Behr, M. (1989). Structure and hierarchy of missing value proportion problems and their representations. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 8(1), 77–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, K. (1981). Ratio and proportion. In K. Hart (Ed.), Children’s understanding of mathematics: 11–16 (pp. 88–101). London, United Kingdom: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffer, A. (1988). Ratios and proportional thinking. In T. Post (Ed.), Teaching mathematics in grades K-8: Research based methods (pp. 285–313). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, L., & Willis, S. (2003). The development of multiplicative thinking in young children. In L. Bragg, C. Campbell, G. Herbert, & J. Mousley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia. Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia: Geelong, Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeong, Y., Levine, S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2007). The development of proportional reasoning: Effect of continuous vs. discrete quantities. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 237–256. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370701202471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J. J., & West, M. M. (1994). Missing-value proportional reasoning problems: Factors affecting informal reasoning patterns. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 235–287). New York, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karplus, R., Pulos, S., & Stage, E. (1983). Proportional reasoning of early adolescents. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematical concepts and processes (pp. 45–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamon, S. J. (1993). Ratio and proportion: Connecting content and children’s thinking. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24, 41–61. https://doi.org/10.2307/749385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamon, S. J. (2008). Teaching fractions and ratios for understanding: Essential content knowledge and instructional strategies for teachers (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesh, R., Post, T., & Behr, M. (1988). Proportional reasoning. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 93–118). Reston, VA: Lawrence Erlbaum & National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (2006). The construction of preference: An overview. In S. Lichtenstein & P. Slovic (Eds.), The construction of preference (pp. 1–40). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618031

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Misailidou, C., & Williams, J. (2003). Children’s proportional reasoning and tendency for an additive strategy: The role of models. Research in Mathematics Education, 5, 215–247. https://doi.org/10.1080/14794800008520123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Modestou, M., & Gagatsis, A. (2010). Cognitive and metacognitive aspects of proportional reasoning. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 12, 36–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986060903465822

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulligan, J. (1992). Children’s solutions to multiplication and division word problems: A longitudinal study. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 4, 24–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03217230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, J. M. C. (2013). Intuition and problem solving. Curriculum and Teaching, 28(2), 27–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noelting, G. (1980). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept: Part 1. Differentiation of stages. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11, 217–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304357

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2010). Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical representation. In T. Nunes, P. Bryant, & A. Watson (Eds.), Key understandings in mathematics learning. London, UK: Nuffield Foundation.

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Barros, R., & Sylva, K. (2012). The relative importance of two different mathematical abilities to mathematical achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 136–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02033.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1982). Analyzing aptitudes for learning: Inductive reasoning. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (vol. 2, pp. 269–345). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B., & Singer, J. A. (1993). Protoquantitative origins of ratio reasoning. In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Rational numbers: An integration of research (pp. 107–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siemon, D., Breed, M., & Virgona, J. (2005). From additive to multiplicative thinking: The big challenge of the middle years. In J. Mousley, L. Bragg, & C. Campbell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd Conference of the Mathematical Association of Victoria (pp. 278–286). Melbourne, Australia: Mathematical Association of Victoria.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P. (1995). The construction of preference. American Psychologist, 50, 364–371. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (2003). Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torbeyns, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2013). Efficient and flexible strategy use on multi-digit sums: A choice/no-choice study. Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 129–140. https://doi.org/10.1080/14794802.2013.797745

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tourniaire, F., & Pulos, S. (1985). Proportional reasoning: A review of the literature. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16, 181–204. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02400937

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vamvakoussi, X., Van Dooren, W., & Verschaffel, L. (2012). Naturally biased? In search for reaction time evidence for a natural number bias in adults. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 31, 344–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2012.02.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., Janssens, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2008). The linear imperative. An inventory and conceptual analysis of students’ overuse of linearity. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 39, 311–342. https://doi.org/10.2307/30034972

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). From addition to multiplication… and back. The development of students’ additive and multiplicative reasoning skills. Cognition and Instruction, 28, 360–381. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2010.488306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., Vleugels, K., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). Just answering... or thinking? Contrasting pupils’ solutions and classifications of proportional and non-proportional word problems. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 12, 20–35. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986060903465806

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., Vamvakoussi, X., & Verschaffel, L. (2018). Proportional reasoning. Educational Practice Series, 30, 1–34.

  • Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematics concepts and processes (pp. 127–174). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, G. (1988). Multiplicative structures. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 141–161). Reston, VA: Lawrence Erlbaum & National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, A., & Mason, J. (2005). Mathematics as a constructive activity: Learners generating examples. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welford, A. T. (1980). Reaction times. London, United Kingdom: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York, NY: Harper and Brothers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Linde Zutterman and Laura Saitta for assisting in data collection.

Funding

This research was partially supported by the C16/16/001 grant “Early development and stimulation of core mathematical competencies” by the Research Council of the KU Leuven and by a postdoctoral fellowship (PDM/19/065) of the KU Leuven.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tine Degrande.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Degrande, T., Verschaffel, L. & Van Dooren, W. To add or to multiply in open problems? Unraveling children’s relational preference using a mixed-method approach. Educ Stud Math 104, 405–430 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-020-09966-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-020-09966-z

Keywords

Navigation