Skip to main content
Log in

Rhythmic movement as a tacit enactment goal mobilizes the emergence of mathematical structures

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article concerns the purpose, function, and mechanisms of students’ rhythmic behaviors as they solve embodied-interaction problems, specifically problems that require assimilating quantitative information structures embedded into the environment. Analyzing multimodal data of one student tackling a bimanual interaction design for proportion, we observed the (1) evolution of coordinated movements with stable temporal–spatial qualities; (2) breakdown of this proto-rhythmic form when it failed to generalize across the problem space; (3) utilization of available resources to obtain greater specificity by way of measuring spatial spans of movements; (4) determination of an arithmetic pattern governing the sequence of spatial spans; and (5) creation of a meta-rhythmic form that reconciles continuous movement with the arithmetic pattern. The latter reconciliation selectively retired, modified, and recombined features of her previous form. Rhythmic enactment, even where it is not functionally imperative, appears to constitute a tacit adaptation goal. Its breakdown reveals latent phenomenal properties of the environment, creating opportunities for quantitative reasoning, ultimately supporting the learning of curricular content.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The interviewer’s question could have been interpreted as relating to a global enactment of continuous movement, yet K responded to the question by initiating local subtasks.

References

  • Abrahamson, D. (2004). Embodied spatial articulation: A gesture perspective on student negotiation between kinesthetic schemas and epistemic forms in learning mathematics. In D. E. McDougall & J. A. Ross (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-sixth annual meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 791–797). Toronto, Ontario: Preney.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D. (2009). Embodied design: Constructing means for constructing meaning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(1), 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D. (2014). Building educational activities for understanding: An elaboration on the embodied-design framework and its epistemic grounds. International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction, 2(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., & Lindgren, R. (2014). Embodiment and embodied design. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (2nd ed., pp. 358–376). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., Lee, R. G., Negrete, A. G., & Gutiérrez, J. F. (2014). Coordinating visualizations of polysemous action: Values added for grounding proportion. ZDM Mathematics Education, 46(1), 79–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., & Trninic, D. (2015). Bringing forth mathematical concepts: Signifying sensorimotor enactment in fields of promoted action. ZDM Mathematics Education, 47(2), 295–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., & Wilensky, U. (2007). Learning axes and bridging tools in a technology-based design for statistics. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 12(1), 23–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberger, J., & diSessa, A. (2003). Music as embodied mathematics: A study of a mutually informing affinity. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 8(2), 123–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bautista, A., & Roth, W.-M. (2012). The incarnate rhythm of geometrical knowing. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 31(1), 91–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin, G. A. (2000). A scientific perspective on structured, task-based interviews in mathematics education research. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 517–545). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A., & Engstrom, D. A. (2006). The complementary nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelton, M. L., & Ma, J. Y. (2018). Reconfiguring mathematical settings and activity through multi-party, whole-body collaboration. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 98(2), 177–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koschmann, T., Kuuti, K., & Hickman, L. (1998). The concept of breakdown in Heidegger, Leont’ev, and Dewey and its implications for education. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 5(1), 25–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leung, A., Baccaglini-Frank, A., & Mariotti, M. A. (2013). Discernment of invariants in dynamic geometry environments. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 84(3), 439–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J. (1989). Mathematical abstraction as the result of a delicate shift of attention. For the Learning of Mathematics, 9(2), 2–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J. (2002). Researching your own practice: The discipline of noticing. London: Routledge Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J. (2010). Attention and intention in learning about teaching through teaching. In R. Leikin & R. Zazkis (Eds.), Learning through teaching mathematics: Development of teachers’ knowledge and expertise in practice (pp. 23–47). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., & Ferrara, F. (2009). Mathematical imagination and embodied cognition. In L. Radford, L. Edwards, & F. Arzarello (Eds.), Gestures and multimodality in the construction of mathematical meaning [special issue]. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 159-174.

  • Nemirovsky, R., Kelton, M. L., & Rhodehamel, B. (2013). Playing mathematical instruments: Emerging perceptuomotor integration with an interactive mathematics exhibit. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 44(2), 372–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, D., Griffin, P., & Cole, M. (1989). The construction zone: Working for cognitive change in school. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1960). The child’s conception of geometry (E.A. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Basic.

  • Radford, L. (2009). Why do gestures matter? Sensuous cognition and the palpability of mathematical meanings. In L. Radford, L. Edwards, & F. Arzarello (Eds.), Gestures and multimodality in the construction of mathematical meaning [special issue]. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 111-126.

  • Radford, L. (2015). Rhythm as an integral part of mathematical thinking. In M. Bockarova, M. Danesi, D. Martinovic, & R. Núñez (Eds.), Mind in mathematics: Essays on mathematical cognition and mathematical method (pp. 68–85). Munich: LINCOM GmbH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Arzarello, F., Edwards, L., & Sabena, C. (2017). The multimodal material mind: Embodiment in mathematics education. In J. Cai (Ed.), First compendium for research in mathematics education (pp. 700–721). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinholz, D., Trninic, D., Howison, M., & Abrahamson, D. (2010). It’s not easy being green: Embodied artifacts and the guided emergence of mathematical meaning. In P. Brosnan, D. Erchick, & L. Flevares (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32 nd annual meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME-NA 32) (Vol. VI, Ch. 18: “Technology,” pp. 1488–1496). Columbus, OH: PME-NA.

  • Richmond, L. L., & Zacks, J. M. (2017). Constructing experience: Event models from perception to action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(12), 962–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, D. M., Palatnik, A., & Abrahamson, D. (2018). A better story: An embodied-design argument for generic manipulatives. In N. Calder, N. Sinclair, & K. Larkin (Eds.), Using mobile technologies in the learning of mathematics (pp. 189–211). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Thom, J. S. (2009). Bodily experience and mathematical conceptions: From classical views to a phenomenological reconceptualization. In L. Radford, L. Edwards, & F. Arzarello (Eds.), Gestures and multimodality in the construction of mathematical meaning [special issue]. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 175-189.

  • Sinclair, N., Chorney, S., & Rodney, S. (2016). Rhythm in number: Exploring the affective, social and mathematical dimensions of using TouchCounts. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 28(1), 31–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmt, E., & Kieren, T. (2015). Three “moves” in enactivist research: A reflection. ZDM Mathematics Education, 47(2), 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, R. M. C., Semjen, A., Yang, S., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). An event-based account of coordination stability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 702–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trninic, D. (Chair), & Saxe, G. (Discussant). (2017, June). Embodied patterns of knowing: Investigating the role of rhythm in cognition and development. Symposium presented at the annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, San Francisco, June 8–10, 2017.

  • Vandervert, L. (2016). The prominent role of the cerebellum in the learning, origin and advancement of culture. Cerebellum & Ataxias, 3(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40673-016-0049-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alik Palatnik.

Ethics declarations

The research program was approved by and strictly complied with the university’s Internal Review Board stipulations.

The research goals were explained to the participants verbally and in printed form. Informed consent was obtained in advance of meetings.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Palatnik, A., Abrahamson, D. Rhythmic movement as a tacit enactment goal mobilizes the emergence of mathematical structures. Educ Stud Math 99, 293–309 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-9845-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-9845-0

Keywords

Navigation