Skip to main content
Log in

Coordinating visualizations of polysemous action: values added for grounding proportion

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We contribute to research on visualization as an epistemic learning tool by inquiring into the didactical potential of having students visualize one phenomenon in accord with two different partial meanings of the same concept. 22 Grade 4–6 students participated in a design study that investigated the emergence of proportional-equivalence notions from mediated perceptuomotor schemas. Working as individuals or pairs in tutorial clinical interviews, students solved non-symbolic interaction problems that utilized remote-sensing technology. Next, they used symbolic artifacts interpolated into the problem space as semiotic means to objectify in mathematical register a variety of both additive and multiplicative solution strategies. Finally, they reflected on tensions between these competing visualizations of the space. Micro-ethnographic analyses of episodes from three paradigmatic case studies suggest that students reconciled semiotic conflicts by generating heuristic logico-mathematical inferences that integrated competing meanings into cohesive conceptual networks. These inferences hinged on revisualizing additive elements multiplicatively. Implications are drawn for rethinking didactical design for proportions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Gestures, too, are dynamical manual actions perceived in the visual modality and bearing semiotic content, yet we will be focusing on pragmatic manual actions operating on objects in the environment and affecting their properties.

  2. These particular students are not featured in the three case studies.

  3. Another resource was an interactive ratio table. When it is layered onto the screen, students effect green by typing numerals into the tables’ cells. This resource is not relevant to the article and so will not be treated further.

  4. In Abrahamson et al. (2012), we present an elaborate table of the interviewers’ tutorial tactics that we determined post facto from analyzing the videography. A list of tutorial tactics is different from an interview protocol, because it describes an instructor’s domain-general dialogical mechanics in service of implementing educational interaction.

  5. See http://tinyurl.com/zdm-viz for video clips from the vignettes.

References

  • Abrahamson, D. (2012). Discovery reconceived: Product before process. For the Learning of Mathematics, 32(1), 8–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., Trninic, D., Gutiérrez, J. F., Huth, J., & Lee, R. G. (2011). Hooks and shifts: A dialectical study of mediated discovery. Technology, Knowledge, and Learning, 16(1), 55–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., Gutiérrez, J. F., Charoenying, T., Negrete, A. G., & Bumbacher, E. (2012). Fostering hooks and shifts: Tutorial tactics for guided mathematical discovery. Technology, Knowledge, and Learning, 17(1–2), 61–86. doi:10.1007/s10758-012-9192-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., & Wilensky, U. (2007). Learning axes and bridging tools in a technology-based design for statistics. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 12(1), 23–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alač, M., & Hutchins, E. (2004). I see what you are saying: Action as cognition in fMRI brain mapping practice. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(3), 629–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arcavi, A. (1994). Symbol sense: Informal sense-making in formal mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, 14(3), 24–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arcavi, A. (2003). The role of visual representations in the learning of mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 52(3), 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arzarello, F., & Robutti, O. (2010). Multimodality in multi-representational environments. In P. Drijvers & H.-G. Wigand (Eds.), The role of handheld technology in the mathematics classroom [Special issue]. ZDM: The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42(7), 715–731.

  • Bakker, A., & Derry, J. (2011). Lessons from inferentialism for statistics education. In K. Makar & D. Ben-Zvi (Eds.), The role of context in developing students’ reasoning about informal statistical inference [Special issue]. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 13(1&2), 5–26.

  • Barwell, R. (2009). Researchers’ descriptions and the construction of mathematical thinking. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 72(2), 255–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bamberger, J., & Ziporyn, E. (1991). Getting it wrong. The World of Music: Ethnomusicology and Music Cognition, 34(3), 22–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1985). Cognitive coping strategies and the problem of “inert knowledge”. In S. F. Chipman, J. W. Segal & R. Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning skills: Current research and open questions [Research and open questions] (Vol. 2, pp. 65–80). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Clinton, K. A. (2006). Being-in-the-digital-world: How videogames engage our pre-linguistic sense-making abilities. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Madison: University of Wisconsin-Madison.

  • Cobb, P., & Steffe, L. (1998). Multiplicative and divisional schemes. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 20(1), 45–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (2000). Analysis of clinical interviews: Foundations and model viability. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 547–589). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J. (1998). Building mathematical structure within a conjecture driven teaching experiment on splitting In S. B. Berenson, K. R. Dawkins, M. Blanton, W. N. Coulombe, J. Kolb, K. Norwood & L. Stiff (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 39–48). Columbus: Eric Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.

  • Confrey, J. (2005). The evolution of design studies as methodology. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 135–151). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, B(366), 1149–1157.

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (2002). Intelligence without representation: Merleau–Ponty’s critique of mental representation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1(4), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J., & Atkins, B. T. S. (2000). Describing polysemy: The case of “crawl”. In C. Leacock (Ed.), Polysemy: Theoretical and computational approaches (pp. 91–110). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foster, C. (2011). Productive ambiguity in the learning of mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, 31(2), 3–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freudenthal, H. (1983). Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P. (1997). Entering the child’s mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Godino, J. D., Font, V., Wilhelmi, M. R., & Lurduy, O. (2011). Why is the learning of elementary arithmetic concepts difficult? Semiotic tools for understanding the nature of mathematical objects. In L. Radford, G. Schubring, & F. Seeger (Eds.), Signifying and meaning-making in mathematical thinking, teaching, and learning: semiotic perspectives [Special issue]. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 77(2), 247–265.

  • Goldin, G. A. (2000). A scientific perspective on structured, task-based interviews in mathematics education research. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 517–545). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Barsalou, L. W. (1998). Reuniting perception and conception. Cognition, 65, 231–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 603–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1982). Epistemological and methodological bases of naturalistic inquiry. Educational Technology Research and Development, 30(4), 233–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacs, E. A., & Clark, H. H. (1987). References in conversations between experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116(26–37).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jay, M. (1988). Scopic regimes of modernity. In H. Foster (Ed.), Vision and visuality (pp. 3–23). New York: The New Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J., & West, M. M. (1994). Missing-value proportional reasoning problems: Factors affecting informal reasoning patterns. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 237–287). Albany: SUNY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. (2010). Thinking with the body. In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 2864–2869). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamon, S. J. (2007). Rational numbers and proportional reasoning: Toward a theoretical framework for research. In F. K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 629–667). Charlotte,: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mamolo, A. (2010). Polysemy of symbols: Signs of ambiguity. The Montana Mathematics Enthusiast, 7(2&3), 247–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 431–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R. (2011). Episodic feelings and transfer of learning. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20(2), 308–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, D., Griffin, P., & Cole, M. (1989). The construction zone: Working for cognitive change in school. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1968). Genetic epistemology (E. Duckworth, Trans.). New York: Columbia University Press.

  • Pirie, S. E. B., & Kieren, T. E. (1994). Growth in mathematical understanding: How can we characterize it and how can we represent it? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2003). Gestures, speech, and the sprouting of signs: A semiotic-cultural approach to students’ types of generalization. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 5(1), 37–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B. (1992). From protoquantities to operators: Building mathematical competence on a foundation of everyday knowledge. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putnam, & R. A. Hattrup (Eds.), Analysis of arithmetic for mathematics teaching (pp. 373–429). Hillsdale: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2010). Incarnation: Radicalizing the embodiment of mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, 30(2), 8–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowland, T. (1999). Pronouns in mathematics talk: Power, vagueness and generalisation. For the Learning of Mathematics, 19(2), 19–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H., Smith, J. P., & Arcavi, A. (1991). Learning: The microgenetic analysis of one student’s evolving understanding of a complex subject matter domain. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 55–175). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2002). The interplay of intimations and implementations: Generating new discourse with new symbolic tools. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 11(2&3), 319–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, R., & Hall, R. (1998). Disciplined perception: Learning to see in technoscience. In M. Lampert & M. L. Blunk (Eds.), Talking mathematics in school: Studies of teaching and learning (pp. 107–149). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures and techniques. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streefland, L. (1993). The design of a mathematics course: A theoretical reflection. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 25(1), 109–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. W. (1993). Quantitative reasoning, complexity, and additive structures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 3, 165–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsal, Y., & Kolbet, L. (1985). Disambiguating ambiguous figures by selective attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(1-A), 25–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematical concepts and processes (pp. 127–174). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dor Abrahamson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Abrahamson, D., Lee, R.G., Negrete, A.G. et al. Coordinating visualizations of polysemous action: values added for grounding proportion. ZDM Mathematics Education 46, 79–93 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-013-0521-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-013-0521-7

Keywords

Navigation