Skip to main content
Log in

Mood Regulation and Cognitive Reactivity in Depression Vulnerability

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is substantial evidence supporting the hypothesis that cognitive reactivity is an important variable in the etiology of depression. However, there is a lack of studies examining possible mechanisms that underlie cognitive reactivity. The present study tested whether two specific mood regulation processes differentially appear in vulnerable and non-vulnerable individuals, and whether they can account for differences in cognitive reactivity. In a cross-sectional experimental design, 20 formerly-depressed individuals (FD) were compared with 20 never-depressed individuals (ND). In an autobiographical memory task both groups differed concerning the use of positively and negatively toned emotion words: FD retrieved fewer positive emotion words than ND in the second phase of this task. Furthermore, FD with a high cognitive reactivity retrieved more negatively toned emotion words. In the ND group there was a different pattern: Subjects with a high cognitive reactivity retrieved less positively toned emotion words. Two different cognitive processes seem to account for cognitive reactivity in individuals who are at high versus low risk for depression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96(2), 358–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bagby, R. M., Ryder, A. G., Schuller, D. R., & Marshall, M. B. (2004). The hamilton depression rating scale: Has the gold standard become a lead weight? The American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(12), 2163–2177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., & Reid, R. M. (2011). Heart rate variability predicts cognitive reactivity to a sad mood provocation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 395–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beevers, C. G., Scott, W. D., McGeary, C., & McGeary, J. E. (2009). Negative cognitive response to a sad mood induction: Associations with polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) gene. Cognition and Emotion, 23(4), 726–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99(2), 229–246.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brewin, C. R., Wheatley, J., Patel, T., Fearon, P., Hackmann, A., Wells, A., et al. (2009). Imagery rescripting as a brief stand-alone treatment for depressed patients with intrusive memories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(7), 569–576.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caseras, X., Garner, M., Bradley, B. P., & Mogg, K. (2007). Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 491–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, R. J. (2000). Ability to disengage attention predicts negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 14(3), 401–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the beck depression inventory—II. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 83–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehring, T., Fischer, S., Schnülle, J., Bösterling, A., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2008). Characteristics of emotion regulation in recovered depressed versus never depressed individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(7), 1574–1584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: Spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion, 10(4), 563–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eizenman, M., Yu, L. H., Grupp, L., Eizenman, E., Ellenbogen, M., Gemar, M., et al. (2003). A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 118(2), 117–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellenbogen, M. A., Schwartzman, A. E., Stewart, J., & Walker, C.-D. (2002). Stress and selective attention: The interplay of mood, cortisol levels, and emotional information processing. Psychophysiology, 39(6), 723–732.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erber, R., & Markunas, S. (2006). Managing affective states. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 253–266). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erber, R., Wegner, D. M., & Therriault, N. (1996). On being cool and collected: Mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(4), 757–766.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, G., Harley, R., Kerrigan, M., Jacobo, M., & Fava, M. (2009). Change in emotional processing during a dialectical behavior therapy-based skills group for major depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(4), 316–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K., Nickel, S., Asbeck, J., & Pagel, U. (2003). Mood and the generation effect. Cognition and Emotion, 17(4), 585–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39–66.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & Ciarrochi, J. V. (2002). On managing moods: Evidence for the role of homeostatic cognitive strategies in affect regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(3), 336–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, E., Prien, R. F., Jarrett, R. B., & Keller, M. B. (1991). Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: Remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48(9), 851–855.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gemar, M. C., Segal, Z. V., Sagrati, S., & Kennedy, S. J. (2001). Mood-induced changes on the implicit association test in recovered depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 282–289.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Cane, D. B. (1987). Construct accessibility and clinical depression: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96(3), 199–204.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grosse Holtforth, M., Wilm, K., Hayes, A., Sutter, M., Schmied, E., & Caspar, F. (submitted). Examining the role of emotional processing in the treatment of depression: A preliminary investigation in exposure-based cognitive therapy (EBCT).

  • Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. M., Feldman, G. C., Beevers, C. G., Laurenceau, J.-P., Cardaciotto, L., & Lewis-Smith, J. (2007). Discontinuities and cognitive changes in an exposure-based cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(3), 409–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognitions in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 52–59.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(5), 443–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Bernet, C. Z., & McLaughlin, S. C. (1994). Attentional allocation processes in individuals at risk for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18(4), 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(2), 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Mood regulation in depression: Differential effects of distraction and recall of happy memories on sad mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 484–490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition and Emotion, 10(4), 437–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, N., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2001). Remitted depression studies as tests of the cognitive vulnerability hypotheses of depression onset: A critique and conceptual analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(1), 63–83.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: Maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Maladaptive cognitive structures in depression. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 135(5), 525–533.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C.-W., & Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der ‘Positive and Negative Affect Schedule’ (PANAS) [Investigations with a German version of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)]. Diagnostica, 42(2), 139–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kühner, C., Bürger, C., Keller, F., & Hautzinger, M. (2007). Reliabilität und Validität des revidierten Beck-Depressions-inventars (BDI-II). Befunde aus deutschsprachigen Stichproben. [Reliability and validity of the Revised Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). Results from German samples.] Der Nervenarzt, 78(6), 651–656.

  • Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10(6), 669–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mergenthaler, E. (1993). TAS/C user manual. Ulm: Ulmer Textbank.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mergenthaler, E. (1996). Emotion—Abstraction patterns in verbatim protocols: A new way of describing psychotherapeutic processes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 1306–1315.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miranda, J., Persons, J. B., & Byers, C. N. (1990). Endorsement of dysfunctional beliefs depends on current mood state. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(3), 237–241.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeiffer, N., Kaemmerer, A., Mearns, J., Catanzaro, S. J., & Backenstrass, M. (2011). Generalized expectancies for negative mood regulation and major depressive disorder: The role of previous depressive episodes and comorbid mental disorders. Psychopathology, 44(3), 152–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rude, S. S., Wenzlaff, R. M., Gibbs, B., Vane, J., & Whitney, T. (2002). Negative processing biases predict subsequent depressive symptoms. Cognition and Emotion, 16(3), 423–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scher, C. D., Ingram, R. E., & Segal, Z. V. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(4), 487–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C. (1994). Incongruent effects of sad mood on self-conception valence: It’s a matter of time. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 161–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., Gemar, M., & Williams, S. (1999). Differential cognitive response to a mood challenge following successful cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy for unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(1), 3–10.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., & Ingram, R. E. (1994). Mood priming and construct activation in tests of cognitive vulnerability to unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 14(7), 663–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., Kennedy, S., Gemar, M., Hood, K., Pedersen, R., & Buis, T. (2006). Cognitive reactivity to sad mood provocation and the prediction of depressive relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(7), 749–755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, J. S., Silverman, J. A., & Eardley, D. A. (1984). Do maladaptive attitudes cause depression? Archives of General Psychiatry, 41(1), 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2(3), 247–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2011). Fixing our focus: Training attention to regulate emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(1), 75–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., & Moulds, M. (2007). Revealing negative thinking in recovered major depression: A preliminary investigation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 3069–3076.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzlaff, R. M., & Bates, D. E. (1998). Unmasking a cognitive vulnerability to depression: How lapses in mental control reveal depressive thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(6), 1559–1571.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, I. M., & Blackburn, I. M. (1981). Cognitive style in depressed and recovered depressed patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20(4), 283–292.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wittchen, H. U., Zaudig, M., & Fydrich, T. (1997). Structural clinical interview for DSM-IV (SKID-I and SKID-II) [in German]. Hogrefe: Göttingen.

Download references

Acknowledgments

The first author was supported by a doctoral grant from the Centre for Psychological Psychotherapy of the University of Heidelberg. The third author was supported by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timo Brockmeyer.

Additional information

Timo Brockmeyer and Nils Pfeiffer contributed equally to the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brockmeyer, T., Pfeiffer, N., Grosse Holtforth, M. et al. Mood Regulation and Cognitive Reactivity in Depression Vulnerability. Cogn Ther Res 36, 634–642 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9406-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9406-7

Keywords

Navigation