Skip to main content
Log in

Good Without Knowing it: Subtle Contextual Cues can Activate Moral Identity and Reshape Moral Intuition

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Intuition is the supra-logic that cuts out all the routine processes of thought and leaps straight from the problem to the answer.—Robert Graves

Abstract

The role of moral intuition (i.e., a set of implicit processes which occur automatically and at the fringe of conscious awareness) has been increasingly implicated in business decisions and (un)ethical business behavior. But troublingly, because implicit processes often operate outside of conscious awareness, decision makers are generally unaware of their influence. We tested whether subtle contextual cues for identity can alter implicit beliefs. In two studies, we found that contextual cues which nonconsciously prime moral identity weaken the implicit association between the categories of “business” and “ethical,” an implicit association which has previously been linked to unethical decision making. Further, changes in this implicit association mediated the relationship between contextually primed moral identity and concern for external stakeholder groups, regardless of self-reported moral identity. Thus, our results show that subtle contextual cues can lead individuals to render more ethical judgments, by automatically restructuring moral intuition below the level of consciousness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In contrast, because explicitly processed (reflective and self-reported) information appears to have little or no effect on implicit processes (Gregg et al. 2006), we expect self-reported moral identity to have little influence on implicit beliefs.

  2. The D algorithm uses as numerator the difference between mean latencies of the two combined tasks. These means included error trials, which incorporated a time penalty because the subject was obliged to complete each trial with the correct response. The numerator difference is divided by an "inclusive" standard deviation computed from all of the subject's latencies in both combined tasks (not a pooled SD for the two tasks). Further computational details are available in Greenwald et al. (2003), which showed that the D algorithm is psychometrically superior to various alternatives.

  3. An alternative dependent measure was constructed by creating a dummy variable with 1 indicating participants listed both parties that are directly (e.g., Gunderall’s stockholders, managers, employees, and suppliers such as RMSC and Raytech) and not directly (e.g., local residents, local natural environment, and animals living in the environment) related to Gunderall’s revenue stream and 0 indicating participants who only listed those parties directly related to Gunderall’s revenue stream. Results suggested that as the strength of the association between business and ethical increases, people are less likely to take into account the interests of parties that are indirectly related to Gunderall. Parallel significant effects were found when counting the total number of stakeholders listed, suggesting the DV of moral concern is robust to variations in measurement conceptualization.

References

  • Aquino, K., Freeman, D., Reed, A., Lim, V. G., & Felps, W. (2009). Testing a social-cognitive model of moral behavior: The interactive influence of situations and moral identity centrality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 123–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aquino, K., McFerran, B., & Laven, M. (2011). Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon moral goodness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aquino, K. F., & Reed, A. I. I. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1423–1440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or would Jesse Jackson fail the implicit association test? Psychological Inquiry, 15, 257–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator variable distinction in social psychological-research—Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Stine, R. A. (1992). Bootstrapping goodness-of-fit measures in structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research, 21, 205–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caza, B., Tiedens, L. Z., & Lee, F. (2011). Power becomes you: The effects of implicit and explicit power on the self-concept. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 114, 15–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation processes: The self-fulfilling nature of automatically-activated stereotypes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, L. M., Graham, J. J., & Flaherty, B. P. (1998). An alternative framework for defining mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice and preference with images of admired group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinh, J. E., & Lord, R. G. (2013). Current trends in moral research what we know and where to go from here. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 380–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, T., & Preston, L. E. (1995). The stakeholder theory of the corporation: Concepts, evidence, and implications. Academy of Management Review, 20, 65–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S., & Pacini, R. (1999). Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro, F., Pfeffer, J., & Sutton, R. I. (2005). Economics language and assumptions: How theories can become self-fulfilling. Academy of Management Review, 30, 8–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, R. E. (1984). Strategic management: A stakeholder approach. Boston, MA: Pitman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, M., & Friedman, R. D. (1962). Capitalism and freedom. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2003). On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 535–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey, P. C. (2005). The relationship between corporate philanthropy and shareholder wealth: A risk management perspective. Academy of Management Review, 30, 777–798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B., & Banaji, M. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: 1. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregg, A. P., Seibt, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Easier done than undone: Asymmetry in the malleability of implicit preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. E. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2002). Dialogue between my head and my heart: Affective influences on moral judgment. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 54–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Bjorklund, F. (2008). Social intuitionists answer six questions about moral psychology. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology: The cognitive science of morality: Intuition and diversity (pp. 181–217). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hekman, D., Aquino, K., Owens, B., Mitchell, T., Schilpzand, P., & Leavitt, K. (2010). An examination of whether and how racial and gender biases influence customer satisfaction ratings. Academy of Management Journal, 53, 238–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hetts, J. J., Sakuma, M., & Pelhman, B. W. (1999). Two roads to positive regard: Implicit and explicit self-evaluation and culture. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 512–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16, 366–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaptein, M. (2004). Business codes of multinational firms: What do they say? Journal of Business Ethics, 50, 15–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, D. Y. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1981). Essays on moral development. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lapsley, K., & Lasky, B. (2001). Prototypic moral character. Identity, 1, 345–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, K., Reynolds, S., Barnes, C. M., Schilpzand, P., & Hannah, S. T. (2012). Different hats, different obligations: Plural occupational identities and situated moral judgments. Academy of Management Journal, 55, 1316–1333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D. E. (2000). Ontogeny, homology, and phylogeny in the Hominid craniofacial skeleton: the problem of the browridge. In P. O’Higgins & M. Cohn (Eds.), Development, growth and evolution: Implications for the study of hominid skeletal evolution (pp. 85–122). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic social prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 842–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, M. A. (2007). The role of other orientation in reactions to job characteristics. Journal of Management, 33, 57–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration Web site. Group Dynamics, 6, 101–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 553–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 1996–2003. Journal of Business Ethics, 59, 375–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, K. R., & Gawronski, B. (2011). Mutual influence between the implicit and explicit self-concepts: The role of memory activation and motivated reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 436–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1998). What happens to the “old” attitude when attitudes change? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Lexington, KY.

  • Pfeffer, J. (1981). Power in Organizations. Marshfield, MA: Pitman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfeffer, J. (1997). Pitfalls on the road to measurement: The dangerous liaison of human resources with the ideas of accounting and finance. Human Resource Management, 36, 357–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeffer, J., & Salancik, G. R. (1978). The external control of organizations: A resource dependence perspective. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piccolo, R. F., & Colquitt, J. A. (2006). Transformational leadership and job behaviors: The mediating role of core job characteristics. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 36, 717–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, A, I. I., & Aquino, K. F. (2003). Moral identity and the expanding circle of moral regard towards out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1270–1284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2006). A neurocognitive model of the ethical decision-making process: implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 737–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. (2008). Moral attentiveness: Who pays attention to moral aspects of life? Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1027–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., Leavitt, K., & Decelles, K. A. (2010). Automatic ethics: The effects of implicit assumptions and contextual cues on moral behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 752–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., Schultz, F. C., & Hekman, D. R. (2006). Balancing stakeholder interests: The constraining effects of resource divisibility and stakeholder saliency. Journal of Business Ethics, 64, 285–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Mackie, D. M., & Strain, L. M. (2006). Of two minds: Forming and changing valence inconsistent attitudes. Psychological Science, 17, 954–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shantz, A., & Latham, G. (2011). The effect of primed goals on employee performance: Implications for human resource management. Human Resource Management, 50, 289–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shao, R., Aquino, K., & Freeman, D. (2008). Beyond moral reasoning: A review of moral identity research and its implications for business ethics. Business Quarterly, 18, 513–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, J. A., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. M. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, L. L., Mazar, N., Gino, F., Ariely, D., & Bazerman, M. H. (2012). Signing at the beginning makes ethics salient and decreases dishonest self-reports in comparison to signing at the end. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(38), 15197–15200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, J. M., & Weiss, E. M. (2002). Studyresponse.com: your online behavioral science research source. http://www.studyresponse.com.

  • Suchman, M. C. (1995). Managing legitimacy: Strategic and institutional approaches. Academy of Management Review, 20, 571–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevino, L. K., Weaver, G. W., & Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Behavioral ethics in organizations: A review. Journal of Management, 32, 951–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann, E. L., Leavitt, K., Menges, J. I., Koopman, J., Howe, M., & Johnson, R. E. (2012). Getting explicit about the implicit: A taxonomy of implicit measures and guide for their use in organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 15(4), 553–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, G. R., Reynolds, S. J., & Brown, M. E. (2013). Moral intuition: Connecting current knowledge to future organizational research and practice. Journal of Management, 40(1), 100–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, S. C., & Petty, R. E. (2001). The effects of stereotype activation on behavior: A review of possible mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 797–826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C. B. (2011). The ethical dangers of deliberative decision making. Administratively Science Quarterly, 56, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Keith Leavitt.

Additional information

Keith Leavitt and Lei Zhu have contributed equally to this work.

Appendix

Appendix

Scenario from Study 2

The following scenario will ask you to put yourself in the place of a manager at Gunderall Corporation, a leader in plastic injection molding and small parts manufacturing.

You have been promoted to a more senior management position within Gunderall Corporation, and are learning more about the Business. Gunderall works with two principle suppliers: the RMSC company out of Dubai which supplies crude oil, and Raytech in Philadelphia which makes all of your product packaging. Recently, you have been hearing rumors that Raytech is having labor disputes with its union. And it is no secret that RMSC uses cheap labor from nearby countries and works them under hard conditions.

Gunderall had their IPO and became a publicly traded company just 6 months ago; thus, the initial stockholders have a lot riding on your decisions and abilities. Because of its rapid growth and unique strategic positioning, Gunderall was able to recruit some very talented young managers. These MBAs could have been making more money had they gone somewhere else, but Gunderall was able to recruit them with greater responsibility than they would have found elsewhere.

Gunderall also employs 1316 people at its home plant in Pittsburgh. Gunderall is viewed as a key employer in an area hit hard by the recession. Most manufacturing plants in the area have closed down, creating extremely high unemployment rates in a traditionally blue-collar town.

Further, Pittsburgh has some moderate issues related to public health and water quality. Although injection molding does not produce the kind of waste the old steel mills did, Gunderall’s products and processes are not exactly ‘environmentally friendly.’

You are starting to realize that as a manager here, you will have a lot of different groups competing for your attention!

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leavitt, K., Zhu, L. & Aquino, K. Good Without Knowing it: Subtle Contextual Cues can Activate Moral Identity and Reshape Moral Intuition. J Bus Ethics 137, 785–800 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2746-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2746-6

Keywords

Navigation