Skip to main content
Log in

Disgusted or Happy, It is not so Bad: Emotional Mini-Max in Unethical Judgments

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although prior work on ethical decision-making has examined the direct impact of magnitude of consequences as well as the direct impact of emotions on ethical judgments, the current research examines the interaction of these two constructs. Building on previous research finding disgust to have a varying impact on ethical judgments depending on the specific behavior being evaluated, we investigate how disgust, as well as happiness and sadness, moderates the effect of magnitude of consequences on an individual’s judgments of another person’s unethical behavior. Specifically, we propose and find that because disgust and happiness are both associated with more heuristic-based processing, they both lead to a stronger reliance on the magnitude of consequences when forming ethical judgments. In contrast, because sad and neutral emotional states are associated with more systematic processing, they both result in a weaker reliance on the magnitude of consequences. As such, the effect of magnitude of consequences on judgments of unethical behaviors is stronger when individuals making the judgments are experiencing disgust or happiness versus sadness or a neutral state. This research shows that ethical judgment severity is contingent on individual-level factors, particularly the current emotional state being experienced by the individual, interacting with magnitude of consequences to impact the ethical decision-making process.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arnaud, A., & Schminke, M. (2012). The ethical climate and context of organizations: A comprehensive model. Organizational Science, 23(6), 1767–1780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, R. P., Gopinath, M., & Nyer, P. U. (1999). The role of emotions in marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 184–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, T. (2001). Dimensions of moral intensity and ethical decision making: An empirical study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(5), 1038–1057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, T., & Valentine, S. (2004). Issue contingencies and marketers’ recognition of ethical issues, ethical judgments and behavioral intentions. Journal of Business Research, 57, 338–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Positive affect and flexibility: Overcoming the precedence of global over local processing of visual information. Motivation and Emotion, 29(2), 123–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beal, D. J., Trougakos, J. P., Weiss, H. M., & Dalal, R. S. (2013). Affect spin and the emotion regulation process at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(4), 593–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H. (2000). The interplay of affect and cognition: The mediating role of general knowledge structures. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 201–222). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(33), 1–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 45–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnkrant, R. E., & Howard, D. J. (1984). Effects of the use of introductory rhetorical questions versus statements on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1218–1230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Callahan, D. (2004). The cheating culture: Why more Americans are doing wrong to get ahead. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. R., Panter, A. T., & Turan, N. (2013). Predicting counterproductive work behavior from guilt proneness. Journal of Business Ethics, 114(1), 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connelly, S., Helton-Fauth, W., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). A managerial in-basket study of the impact of trait emotions on ethical choice. Journal of Business Ethics, 51(3), 245–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craft, J. L. (2013). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 2004–2011. Journal of Business Ethics, 117(2), 221–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cryder, C. E., Lerner, J. S., Gross, J. J., & Dahl, R. E. (2008). Misery is not miserly: Sad and self-focused individuals spend more. Psychological Science, 19(6), 525–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. B. (2006). Are audit-related ethical decisions dependent upon mood? Journal of Business Ethics, 68(2), 191–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., & Sweitzer, V. L. (2008). Moral disengagement in ethical decision making: A study of antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 374–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(6), 1055–1067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eyal, T., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). Judging near and distant virtue and vice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1204–1209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, R. C., & Richardson, W. D. (1994). Ethical decision making: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Business Ethics, 13(3), 205–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franke, G. R., Crown, D. F., & Spake, D. F. (1997). Gender differences in ethical perceptions of business practices: A social role theory perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(6), 920–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. F. (2000). The impact of moral intensity on decision making in a business context. Journal of Business Ethics, 26(3), 181–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel, A. S., Diefendorff, J. M., & Erickson, R. J. (2011). The relations of daily task accomplishment satisfaction with changes in affect: A multilevel study in nurses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(5), 1095–1104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garg, N., Wansink, B., & Inman, J. J. (2007). The influence of incidental affect on consumers’ food intake. Journal of Marketing, 71(1), 194–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaudine, A., & Thorne, L. (2001). Emotion and ethical decision-making in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 31(2), 175–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M. (1989). Mood and absence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(2), 317–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1992). Feeling good-doing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood at work-organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 310–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster creativity and good ones don’t: The role of context and clarity of feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 687–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2008). Blinded by anger or feeling the love: How emotions influence advice taking. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(5), 1165–1173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Shu, L. L., & Bazerman, M. H. (2010). Nameless + harmless = blameless: When seemingly irrelevant factors influence judgment of (un)ethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111(2), 93–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852–870). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollings, James. (2013). Let the story go: The role of emotion in the decision-making process of the reluctant, vulnerable witness or whistle-blower. Journal of Business Ethics, 114(3), 501–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 963–976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. (1986). A general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of Macromarketing, 6(1), 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutcherson, C. A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The moral emotions: A social–functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 179–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (2001). An influence of positive affect on decision making in complex situations: Theoretical issues with practical implications. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(2), 75–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (2004). Some perspectives on positive feelings and emotions: Positive affect facilitates thinking and problem solving. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 263–281). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., & Baron, R. A. (1991). Positive affect in organizations. In L. Cummings & B. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (pp. 1–52). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. The Academy of Management Review, 16, 366–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H., & Ryan, C. S. (2009). Data analysis: A model comparison approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2004). Affect and job satisfaction: A study of their relationship at work and at home. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(4), 661–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J.-E., & Johnson, K. P. (2013). The impact of moral emotions on cause-related marketing campaigns: A cross-cultural examination. Journal of Business Ethics, 112(1), 79–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraiger, K., Billings, R. S., & Isen, A. M. (1989). The influence of positive affective states on task perceptions and satisfaction. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 12–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labroo, A. A., & Patrick, V. M. (2009). Psychological distancing: Why happiness helps you see the big picture. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 800–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and purse strings: Carry-over effects of emotions on economic transactions. Psychological Science, 15, 337–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. (1989). Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(1), 27–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • May, D. R., & Pauli, K. P. (2002). The role of moral intensity in ethical decision making: A review and investigation of moral recognition, evaluation, and intention. Business and Society, 41(1), 84–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMahon, J., & Harvey, R. (2007). The effect of moral intensity on ethical judgment. Journal of Business Ethics, 72, 335–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mencl, J., & May, D. R. (2009). The effects of proximity and empathy on ethical decision-making: An exploratory investigation. Journal of Business Ethics, 85(2), 201–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, S., & McDonald, R. (1995). The role of moral intensity in moral judgments: An empirical investigation. Journal of Business Ethics, 14(9), 715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 1996–2003. Journal of Business Ethics, 59(4), 375–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paolillo, J. G. P., & Vitell, S. J. (2002). An empirical investigation of the influence of selected personal, organization, and moral intensity factors on ethical decision making. Journal of Business Ethics, 35(1), 65–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Heesacker, M. (1981). Effects of rhetorical questions on persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 432–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(1), 56–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2006). A neurocognitive model of the ethical decision-making process: Implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 737–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., & Ceranic, T. L. (2007). The effects of moral judgment and moral identity on moral behavior: An empirical examination of the moral individual. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1610–1624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J., Leavitt, K., & DeCelles, K. A. (2010). Automatic ethics: The effects of implicit assumptions and contextual cues on moral behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 752–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, D. C., & Ross, W. T., Jr. (1995). Decision-making processes on ethical issues: The impact of a social contract perspective. Business Ethics Quarterly, 5(2), 213–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romani, S., Grappi, S., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2013). Explaining consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility: The role of gratitude and altruistic values. Journal of Business Ethics, 114(2), 193–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, W. T., Jr., & Robertson, D. C. (2000). Lying: The impact of decision context. Business Ethics Quarterly, 10(2), 409–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, W. T., Jr., & Robertson, D. C. (2003). A typology of situational factors: Impact on salesperson decision-making about ethical issues. Journal of Business Ethics, 46(3), 213–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. (2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1096–1109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (2002). Situated cognition and the wisdom of feelings: Cognitive tuning. In L. F. Barrett & P. Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom in feelings (pp. 144–166). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgment (pp. 55–71). Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selart, M., & Johansen, S. T. (2011). Ethical decision making in organizations: The role of leadership stress. Journal of Business Ethics, 99(2), 129–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singhapakdi, A., Vitell, S. J., & Kraft, K. L. (1996). Moral intensity and ethical decision-making of marketing professionals. Journal of Business Research, 36(3), 245–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2008). “Did you have a nice evening?” A day-level study on recovery experiences, sleep, and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(3), 674–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1993). Affect and managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 304–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., & Dutton, J. E. (1981). Threat rigidity effects in organizational behavior: A multilevel analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26, 501–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treviño, L. K., Weaver, G. R., & Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Behavioral ethics in organizations: A review. Journal of Management, 32, 951–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, S., & Barnett, T. (2002). Ethics codes and sales professionals’ perceptions of their organizations’ ethical values. Journal of Business Ethics, 40(3), 191–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watley, L. D., & May, D. R. (2004). Enhancing moral intensity: The roles of personal and consequential information in ethical decision-making. Journal of Business Ethics, 50(2), 105–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, J. (1996). Influences upon managerial moral decision making: Nature of the harm and magnitude of consequences. Human Relations, 49, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, H., Nicholas, J. P., & Daus, C. S. (1999). An examination of the joint effects of affective experiences and job beliefs on job satisfaction and variations in affective experiences over time. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16, 780–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, F. A., Charles, M. A., & Nelson, J. K. (2008). The role of persuasive arguments in changing affirmative action attitudes and expressed behavior in higher education. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1271–1286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2011). Incidental anger and the desire to evaluate. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116(1), 55–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winterich, K. P., Aquino, K., Mittal, V., & Swartz, R. (2013). When moral identity symbolization motivates prosocial behavior: The role of recognition and moral identity internalization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(5), 759–770.

  • Winterich, K. P., Han, S., & Lerner, J. S. (2010). Now that I’m sad, It’s hard to be mad: The role of cognitive appraisals in emotional blunting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(11), 1467–1483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winterich, K. P., & Haws, K. L. (2011). Helpful hopefulness: The effect of future positive emotions on consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 38, 505–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Rajan Sambandam and Christi Clark at TRC, Inc., Fort Washington, PA, for facilitating data collection through their online panel. Other studies for this project were collected in the behavioral research labs at Arizona State University and Pennsylvania State University. We appreciate feedback from Ajay Kalra, William T. Ross, Linda Trevino, and Kathleen Vohs on earlier versions of this manuscript and assistance from Nivriti Chowdhry, Jihye Jung, and Tatianna Aker.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karen Page Winterich.

Appendices

Appendix: Instructions for Emotion Induction in Studies 1A, 1B and 2

Neutral

Provide as much detail as you can and take your time, but you do not need to spend more than 2–4 min on this question.

Question 1: What are the 3–5 activities that you did today? Please write 2–3 sentences about each thing that you select. Examples of things you might write about include: getting ready in the morning, eating lunch, going to work, running errands, etc.

Now we’d like you to take a minute to think about the activities that typically occur in your evening and determine the way that you most typically spend your evening. Once you have carefully considered your typical evening, please continue to the next page.

You do not need to take more than 3–5 min to complete this question.

Question 2: Thinking about the way you typically spend your evening, please begin by writing down a description of your activities, and then figure out how much time you devoted to each activity. Examples of things you might describe include preparing dinner, eating dinner, spending time with family or friends, running errands, watching TV, etc.

IF YOU CAN, PLEASE WRITE YOUR DESCRIPTION SO THAT SOMEONE READING THIS MIGHT BE ABLE TO RECONSTRUCT THE WAY IN WHICH YOU, SPECIFICALLY, SPEND YOUR EVENINGS.

Disgust

Provide as much detail as you can and take your time, but you do not need to spend more than 2–4 min on this question.

Question 1: What are the 3–5 things that make you find most disgusting? Please write 2–3 sentences about each thing that makes you disgusted. Disgust often arises when you experience something very unpleasant with an aversive physical reaction to avoid the stimulus and perhaps even nausea. Examples of things you might write about include: using a dirty toilet, seeing a dead animal, eating spoiled food, etc.

Now we’d like you to take a minute to think about each of the situations described above and determine the one experience that has been the most disgusting experience of your life. Once you have carefully considered this situation, please continue to the next page.

You do not need to take more than 3–5 min to complete this question.

Question 2: Thinking about the one experience that has been the most disgusting in your life, please begin by writing down what you remember of the disgusting event, and continue by writing as detailed a description of the event as possible. You may want to close your eyes as you recall this memory so you can visualize how things looked, smelled, and/or tasted. Please try to imagine the event as vividly as possible and relive the feelings you experienced at the time of this event.

IF YOU CAN, PLEASE WRITE YOUR DESCRIPTION SO THAT SOMEONE READING THIS MIGHT FEEL PHYSICALLY DISGUSTED JUST FROM LEARNING ABOUT THE SITUATION.

What is it like to be in this situation? What did the situation look like? What smells or tastes were there? Why does it make you so disgusted?

Sadness

Provide as much detail as you can and take your time, but you do not need to spend more than 2–4 min on this question.

Question 1: What are the 3–5 things that make you the most unhappy? Please write 2–3 sentences about each thing that makes you unhappy. Unhappiness often arises when you experience something very unenjoyable or sad. Examples of things you might write about include: the loss of a loved one, devastation from natural disasters, learning of deplorable events on the news, being filled with grief from loss, etc.

Now we’d like you to take a minute to think about each of the situations described above and determine the one day that has been the unhappiest day of your life. Once you have carefully considered this situation, please continue to the next page.

You do not need to take more than 3–5 min to complete this question.

Question 2: Thinking about the one day that has been the unhappiest day of your life, please begin by writing down what you remember of the unhappy event(s), and continue by writing as detailed a description of the event(s) as possible. You may want to close your eyes as you recall this memory so you can visualize how you felt. Please try to imagine the event as vividly as possible and relive the feelings you experienced at the time of this event.

IF YOU CAN, PLEASE WRITE YOUR DESCRIPTION SO THAT SOMEONE READING THIS MIGHT FEEL UNHAPPY ON YOUR BEHALF JUST FROM LEARNING ABOUT THE SITUATION.

What is it like to be in this situation? Why does it make you so unhappy?

Happiness

Provide as much detail as you can and take your time, but you do not need to spend more than 2–4 min on this question.

Question 1: What are the 3–5 things that make you the happiest? Please write 2–3 sentences about each thing that makes you happy. Happiness often arises when you experience something you really enjoy or take pleasure in everyday events. Examples of things you might write about include: receiving a gift from a loved one unexpectedly, enjoying the sunshine on a beautiful day, spending time with your family or friends, reading a good book or playing a game you enjoy, etc.

Now we would like you to take a minute to think about each of the situations described above and determine the 1 day that has been the happiest day of your life. Once you have carefully considered this situation, please continue to the next page.

You do not need to take more than 3–5 min to complete this question.

Question 2: Thinking about the 1 day that has been the happiest day of your life, please begin by writing down what you remember of the happiness-inducing event(s), and continue by writing as detailed a description of the event(s) as possible. You may want to close your eyes as you recall this memory so you can visualize how you felt. Please try to imagine the event as vividly as possible and relive the feelings you experienced at the time of this event.

IF YOU CAN, PLEASE WRITE YOUR DESCRIPTION SO THAT SOMEONE READING THIS MIGHT FEEL HAPPY ON YOUR BEHALF JUST FROM LEARNING ABOUT THE SITUATION.

What is it like to be in this situation? Why does it make you so happy?

Unethical Behavior Scenarios for Studies 1A and 1B

High (Low) Magnitude of Consequences

  1. 1.

    Terri is a financial advisor and has 20 clients who are successful small business owners. Each business owner is investing about $500,000 with her. This represents virtually all of the retirement funds for each business. She started to report lower returns to her clients than she was actually making from the investments. As her business grew, her returns increased and the differences that she reported to her clients increased as well. Over the course of 10 years, she stole over $10 million ($1,000) from her clients.

  2. 2.

    Steve is completing his annual tax return. Every year he donates $1,000 to charity. He is eligible for a $1,000 tax credit because of his donations. This year he reported donating $20,000 ($2,000) so he’ll receive a $20,000 ($2,000) tax credit even though he actually donated $1,000.

  3. 3.

    Jim has insurance on his family’s jewelry that is worth $100,000 ($1,000). He is running short on cash so he sells his family’s jewelry to a local pawn shop for $100 ($80,000). He then makes an insurance claim that his family lost some of their jewelry so the insurance company pays him to replace his jewelry.

  4. 4.

    Bob is a 30-year old male with an annual salary of $55,000 as an accountant for a Fortune 500 company. When he does his payroll, he sometimes adds some extra days of pay. He has stolen $25,000 ($100) from the organization in the few years he has worked there.

Unethical Behavior Scenarios for Study 2

High Magnitude of Consequences

  1. 1.

    Your plane has crashed in the Himalayas. The only survivors are yourself, another man, and a young boy. The three of you travel for days, battling extreme cold and wind. Your only chance at survival is to find your way to a small village on the other side of the mountain, several days away. The boy has a broken leg and cannot move very quickly. His chances of surviving the journey are essentially zero. Without food, you and the other man will probably die as well. The other man suggests that you sacrifice the boy and eat his remains over the next few days (Schnall et al. 2008).

  2. 2.

    Matthew is playing with his new kitten late one night. He is wearing only his boxer shorts, and the kitten sometimes walks over his genitals. Eventually, this arouses him, and he begins to rub his bare genitals along the kitten’s body. The kitten purrs, and seems to enjoy the contact (Schnall et al. 2008).

  3. 3.

    A brother and sister are alone in the house and decide to make love just once. The sister is already taking birth control pills and the brother uses a condom. They both enjoy the act (Eyal et al. 2008).

  4. 4.

    The neighbors’ dog is hit by a car in front of their house and is killed. A member of the neighbors’ family has heard that dog meat is delicious, and suggests cooking it and eating it for dinner (Eyal et al. 2008).

Low Magnitude of Consequences (Detert et al. 2008)

  1. 1.

    Sam works in a fast-food restaurant. It is against policy to eat food without paying for it. Sam comes straight from classes and is, therefore, hungry. Sam’s supervisor isn’t around, so he makes something for himself and eats it without paying.

  2. 2.

    Josh waited in line for 10 min to buy a coffee and muffin at the coffee shop. When he’s a couple blocks away, he realizes that the clerk gave him change for $20 rather than for the $10 he gave him. He savors the coffee, muffin and free $10.

  3. 3.

    Todd gets the final exam back from his professor and you notice that he is marked correct three answers that he got wrong. Revealing the professor’s error would mean the difference between an A and a B. Todd says nothing.

  4. 4.

    Steph’s boss at her summer job asks her to get confidential information about a competitor’s product. She, therefore, poses as a student doing a research project on the competitor’s company and asks for the information.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Winterich, K.P., Morales, A.C. & Mittal, V. Disgusted or Happy, It is not so Bad: Emotional Mini-Max in Unethical Judgments. J Bus Ethics 130, 343–360 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2228-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2228-2

Keywords

Navigation