Skip to main content
Log in

Genetic and Environmental Sources of Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem and Affect: Results from a Genetically Sensitive Multi-group Design

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In today’s world, researchers frequently utilize indirect measures of implicit (i.e., automatic, spontaneous) evaluations. The results of several studies have supported the usefulness of these measures in predicting behavior, as compared to utilizing direct measures of explicit (i.e., purposeful, deliberate) evaluations. A current, under-debate issue concerns the origin of these implicit evaluations. The present genetically sensitive multi-group study analyzed data from 223 twin pairs and 222 biological core families to estimate possible genetic and environmental sources of individual differences in implicit and explicit self-esteem and affect. The results show that implicit self-esteem and affect maintain a substantial genetic basis, but demonstrate little influence from the shared environment by siblings (e.g., shared familial socialization in childhood). A bivariate analysis found that implicit and explicit evaluations of the same construct share a common genetic core which aligns with the motivation and opportunity as determinants (MODE) model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In line with De Houwer (2005), we use the terms direct and indirect for measurement procedures and the terms explicit and implicit for the measurement outcome.

  2. A further frequently used term is “name letter test.”

  3. In a strict sense, genetic and environmental components are only error-free as long as the error itself is randomly distributed and has no genetic and environmental sources.

  4. All values for kurtosis (rescaled to zero) and skewness were <|1.2| except for the signature height (kurtosis = 2.7). Nevertheless all values were in an acceptable range regarding recommendations of Bentler (2006; ±3) and Byrne (2010; ±5).

  5. We have also calculated bivariate models for significant correlations between indirect measures (IPT vs. GNL; IPA vs. INA), between direct measures of the same construct (EPA vs. ENA), as well as across psychological concepts (RSES vs. EPA, RSES vs. ENA, IPT vs. EPA, GNL vs. EPA, IPT vs. ENA, GNL vs. ENA). Results can be found in the online supplement.

References

  • Arbuckle JL (2013) AMOS user’s guide 22.0. SPSS, Chicago

  • Baccus JR, Baldwin MW, Packer DJ (2004) Increasing implicit self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychol Sci 15:498–502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Back MD, Krause S, Hirschmüller S, Stopfer J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC (2009) Unraveling the three faces of self-esteem: a new information-processing sociometer perspective. J Res Personal 43:933–937

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker LA, Cesa IL, Gatz M, Mellins C (1992) Genetic and environmental influences on positive and negative affect: support for a two-factor theory. Psychol Aging 7:158–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler PM (2006) EQS 6 structural equations program manual. Multivariate Software Inc, Encino

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanton H, Jaccard J (2015) Not so fast: ten challenges to importing implicit attitude measures to media psychology. Med Psychol 18:338–369

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boardman JD, Blalock CL, Button TMM (2008) Sex differences in the heritability of resilience. Twin Res Hum Genet 11:12–27

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bønnelykke B, Hauge M, Holm N, Kristoffersen K, Gurtler H (1989) Evaluation of zygosity diagnosis in twin pairs below age seven by means of a mailed questionnaire. Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) 38:305–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosson JK, Swann WB, Pennebaker JW (2000) Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: the blind men and the elephant revisited? J Personal Soc Psychol 79:631–643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard TJ, Loehlin JC (2001) Genes, evolution, and personality. Behav Genet 31:243–273

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown KW, Ryan RM (2003) The benefits of being present: the role of mindfulness in psychological well-being. J Personal Soc Psychol 84:822–848

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester MD, Blanton H, Swann WB (2011) Implicit self-esteem: nature, measurement, and a new way forward. J Personal Soc Psychol 100:365–385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne BM (2010) Structural equation modeling with AMOS: basic concepts, applications, and programming. Routledge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Castelli L, Carraro L, Gawronski B, Gava K (2010) On the determinants of implicit evaluations: when the present weighs more than the past. J Exp Soc Psychol 46:186–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli L, Zogmeister C, Tomelleri S (2009) The transmission of racial attitudes within the family. Develop Psychol 45:586–591

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken S, Trope Y (eds) (1999) Dual-process models in social psychology. Guilford, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham WA, Preacher KJ, Banaji MR (2001) Implicit attitude measures: consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychol Sci 12:163–170

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer J (2005) What are implicit and indirect measures of attitudes? Soc Psychol Rev 7:18–20

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer J, Teige-Mocigemba S, Spruyt A, Moors A (2009) Implicit measures: a normative analysis and review. Psychol Bull 135:347–368

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeHart T, Pelham BW, Tennen H (2006) What lies beneath: parenting style and implicit self-esteem. J Exp Soc Psychol 42:1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis A (2004) I like myself but I don’t know why: enhancing implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. J Personal Soc Psychol 86:345–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis A, Albers LW, Bongers KCA (2009) Digging for the real attitude: Lessons from research on implicit and explicit self-esteem. In: Petty RE, Fazio RH, Briñol P (eds) Attitudes: insights from the new implicit measures. Psychology Press, New York, pp 229–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Egloff B, Schmukle SC (2002) Predictive validity of an implicit association test for assessing anxiety. J Personal Soc Psychol 83:1441–1455

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S, Morling B (1995) Is the self motivated to do more than enhance and/or verify itself? In: Kernis MH (ed) Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem. Plenum, New York, pp 9–29

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio FH (1990) Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: the MODE model as an integrative framework. In: Zanna MP (ed) Advances in experimental psychology. Academic Press, New York, pp 75–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio RH, Sanbonmatsu DM, Powell MC, Kardes FR (1986) On the automatic activation of attitudes. J Personal Soc Psychol 50:229–238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski B (2002) What does the implicit association test measure? A test of the convergent and discriminant validity of prejudice-related IATs. Exp Psychol 49:171–180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski B, Bodenhausen GV (2006) Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol Bull 132:692–731

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski B, LeBel EP (2008) Understanding patterns of attitude change: when implicit measures show change, but explicit measures do not. J Exp Soc Psychol 44:1355–1361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski B, Sritharan R (2010) Formation, change, and contextualization of mental associations. In: Gawronski B, Payne BK (eds) Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford, New York, pp 216–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski B, Deutsch R, Banse R (2011) Response interference tasks as indirect measures of automatic associations. In: Klauer KC, Voss A, Stahl C (eds) Cognitive methods in social psychology. Guilford, New York, pp 78–123

    Google Scholar 

  • Gebauer JE, Riketta M, Broemer P, Maio GR (2008) How much do you like your name? An implicit measure of global self-esteem. J Exp Soc Psychol 44:1346–1354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, Banaji MR (1995) Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev 102:4–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, Farnham SD (2000) Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. J Personal Soc Psychol 79:1022–1038

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JLK (1998) Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Personal Soc Psychol 74:1464–1480

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann EL, Banaji MR (2009) Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. J Personal Soc Psychol 97:17–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregg AP, Sedikides C (2010) Narcissistic fragility: rethinking its links to explicit and implicit self-esteem. Self Identity 9:142–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn E, Spinath FM, Siedler T, Wagner GG, Schupp J, Kandler C (2012) The complexity of personality: advantages of a genetically sensitive multi-group design. Behav Genet 42:221–233

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann W, Gawronski B, Gschwendner T, Le H, Schmitt M (2005) A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 31:1369–1385

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoorens V (2014) What’s really in a name-letter effect? Name-letter preferences as indirect measures of self-esteem. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 25:228–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoorens V, Takano K, Franck E, Roberts JE, Raes F (2015) Initial and noninitial name-letter preferences as obtained through repeated letter rating tasks continue to reflect (different aspects of) self-esteem. Psychol Assess 27:905–914

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson RW, Snieder H, Davis H, Treiber FA (2001) Determination of twin zygosity: a comparison of DNA with various questionnaire indices. Twin Res 4:12–18

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan CH, Spencer SJ, Zanna MP, Hoshino-Browne E, Correll J (2003) Secure and defensive high self-esteem. J Personal Soc Psychol 85:969–978

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamakura T, Ando J, Ono Y (2001) Genetic and environmental influences on self-esteem in a Japanese twin sample. Twin Res 4:439–442

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kandler C, Zimmermann J, McAdams DP (2014) Core and surface characteristics for the description and theory of personality differences and development. Eur J Personal 28:231–243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher N (2010) Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107:11163–11170

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA (1998) A population-based twin study of self-esteem and gender. Psychol Med 28:1403–1409

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama S, Karasawa M (1997) Implicit self-esteem in Japan: name letters and birthday numbers. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 23:736–742

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koole SL, Dijksterhuis A, van Knippenberg A (2001) What’s in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. J Personal Soc Psychol 80:669–685

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause S, Back MD, Egloff B, Schmukle SC (2011) Reliability of implicit self-esteem measures revisited. Eur J Personal 25:239–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krizan Z (2008) What is implicit about implicit self-esteem? J Res Personal 42:1635–1640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krizan Z, Suls J (2008) Are implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem related? A meta-analysis for the Name-Letter Test. Personal Indiv Diff 44:521–531

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krohne HW, Egloff B, Kohlmann C-W, Tausch A (1996) Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der “Positive and Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS) [Investigations with a German version of the “Positive and Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS)]. Diagnostica 42:139–156

    Google Scholar 

  • LeBel EP, Gawronski B (2009) How to find what’s in a name: scrutinizing the optimality of five scoring algorithms for the name-letter task. Eur J Personal 23:85–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little RJA, Rubin DB (2002) Statistical analysis with missing data, 2nd edn. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin JC, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D (2003) The behavior genetics of personality and the NEAD study. J Res Pers 37:373–387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luo YLL, Shi Y, Cai H, Wu M, Song H (2014) Liking for name predicts happiness: a behavioral genetic analysis. Pers Indiv Diff 69:156–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan C, Murray HA (1935) A method for investigating fantasies: the thematic apperception test. Arch Neurol 34:289–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neiss MB, Sedikides C, Stevenson J (2002) Self-esteem: a behavioral genetic perspective. Eur J Personal 16:351–367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neiss MB, Stevenson J, Sedikides C, Kumashiro M, Finkel EJ, Rusbult CE (2005) Executive self, self-esteem, and negative affectivity: relations at the phenotypic and genotypic level. J Personal Soc Psychol 89:593–606

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neiss MB, Sedikides C, Stevenson J (2006) Genetic influences on level and stability of self-esteem. Self Identity 5:247–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek BA, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR (2007) The Implicit Association Test at age 7: a methodological and conceptual review. In: Bargh JA (ed) Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior. Psychology Press, New York, pp 265–292

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosek BA, Hawkins CB, Frazier RS (2011) Implicit social cognition: from measures to mechanisms. Trends Cogn Sci 15:152–159

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nuttin JM (1985) Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: the name letter effect. Eur J Soc Psychol 15:353–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson KR, Dunham Y (2010) The development of implicit social cognition. In: Gawronski B, Payne BK (eds) Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford, New York, pp 241–254

    Google Scholar 

  • Orne MT (1962) On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. Am Psychol 17:776–783

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osinsky R, Alexander N, Schmitz A, Kuepper Y, Müller E, Beer L, Koeppe L, Hennig J (2010) Genetic influences on implicit measures of personality. J Indiv Diff 31:115–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham BW, Koole SL, Hardin CD, Hetts JJ, Seah E, De Hart T (2005) Gender moderates the relation between implicit and explicit self-esteem. J Exp Soc Psychol 41:84–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R, Corley R, Caspi A, Fulker DW, DeFries J (1998) Adoption results for self-reported personality: evidence for nonadditive genetic effects? J Personal Soc Psychol 75:211–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quirin M, Kazén M, Kuhl J (2009) When nonsense sounds happy or helpless: the Implicit Positive And Negative Affect Test (IPANAT). J Personal Soc Psychol 97:500–516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quirin M, Wróbel M, Pala AN, Stieger S, Shanchuan D, Hicks JA et al (2016) A cross-cultural validation of the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT): results from ten nations across three continents. Eur J Psychol Assess. doi:10.1027/1015-5759/a000315

  • Raevuori A, Dick DM, Keski-Rahkonen A, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ, Rissanen A, Kaprio J, Viken RJ, Silventoinen K (2007) Genetic and environmental factors affecting self-esteem from age 14 to 17: a longitudinal study of Finnish twins. Psychol Med 37:1625–1633

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roy M-A, Neale MC, Kendler KS (1995) The genetic epidemiology of self-esteem. Br J Psychiatry 166:813–820

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman LA (2004) Sources of implicit attitudes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 13:79–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman LA, Dohn MC, Fairchild K (2007a) Implicit self-esteem compensation: automatic threat defense. J Personal Soc Psychol 93:798–813

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman LA, Phelan JE, Heppen JB (2007b) Developmental sources of implicit attitudes. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 33:1700–1713

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph A, Schröder-Abé M, Schütz A, Gregg AP, Sedikides C (2008) Through a glass, less darkly? Reassessing convergent and discriminant validity in measures of implicit self-esteem. Eur J Psychol Assess 24:273–281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sakellaropoulo M, Baldwin MW (2007) The hidden sides of self-esteem: two dimensions of implicit self-esteem and their relation to narcissistic reactions. J Exp Soc Psychol 43:995–1001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sekaquaptewa D, Vargas P, von Hippel W (2010) A practical guide to paper-and-pencil implicit measures of attitudes. In: Gawronski B, Payne BK (eds) Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford, New York, pp 140–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman JW, Klauer KC, Allen TJ (2010) Mathematical modeling of implicit social cognition: the machine in the ghost. In: Gawronski B, Payne BK (eds) Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford, New York, pp 156–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair S, Dunn E, Lowery BS (2005) The relationship between parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. J Exp Soc Psychol 41:283–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith ER, DeCoster J (2000) Heuristic-systematic and other dual process models in social and cognitive psychology: an integration and connectionist interpretation. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 4:108–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder CR, Fromkin HL (1977) Abnormality as a positive characteristic: the development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. J Abnorm Psychol 86:518–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stieger S, Burger C (2010) Implicit and explicit self-esteem in the context of Internet addiction. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 13:681–688

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stieger S, Burger C (2013) More complex than previously thought: new insights into the optimal administration of the Initial Preference Task. Self Identity 12:201–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stieger S, LeBel EP (2012) Name-letter preferences for new last name and abandoned birth name initials in the context of name-change via marriage. Soc Psychol 43:7–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stieger S, Voracek M, Formann AK (2012) How to administer the Initial Preference Task. Eur J Personal 26:63–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack F, Deutsch R (2004) Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 8:220–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarlow EM, Haaga DAF (1996) Negative self-concept: specificity to depressive symptoms and relation to positive and negative affectivity. J Res Personal 30:120–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teige-Mocigemba S, Klauer KC, Sherman JW (2010) A practical guide to implicit association tests and related tasks. In: Gawronski B, Payne BK (eds) Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford, New York, pp 117–139

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesser A (2000) On the confluence of self-esteem maintenance mechanisms. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 4:290–299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann E, Swanson J (2004) Exposure to violent video games increases automatic aggressiveness. J Adolesc 27:41–52

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Collani G, Herzberg PY (2003) Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg [A revision of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in German language]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 24:3–7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voracek M, Dressler SG (2007) Digit ratio (2D:4D) in twins: heritability estimates and evidence for a masculinized trait expression in women from opposite-sex pairs. Psychol Rep 100:115–126

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wentura D, Degner J (2010) A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In: B Gawronski, BK Payne (eds) Handbook of implicit social cognition. Measurement, theory, and applications. Guilford, New York, pp 95–116

  • Zhu G, Song Y, Hu S, Li X, Tian M, Zhen Z, Dong Q, Liu J (2009) Heritability of the specific cognitive ability of face perception. Curr Biol 20:1–6

    Google Scholar 

  • Zweigenhaft RL (1977) The empirical study of signature size. Soc Behav Personal 5:177–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefan Stieger.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors Stefan Stieger, Christian Kandler, Ulrich S. Tran, Jakob Pietschnig, Martin Voracek declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Edited by Deborah Finkel.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 329 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stieger, S., Kandler, C., Tran, U.S. et al. Genetic and Environmental Sources of Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem and Affect: Results from a Genetically Sensitive Multi-group Design. Behav Genet 47, 175–192 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-016-9829-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-016-9829-8

Keywords

Navigation