Skip to main content
Log in

Emotional faces influence numerosity estimation without awareness

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstracts

It is well established that emotional information influences perceived duration. On the basis of this together with the proposition of common magnitude estimation system, we hypothesized that the presentation of emotional faces can modulate the estimation of numerical quantity as well. The present study examined this hypothesis by investigating the effects of unconsciously presented emotional faces on numerosity estimation using number bisection task. We rendered the facial images invisible by continuous flash suppression methodology in order to measure the genuine effect of emotional information by reducing the influences of a conscious effort of emotional regulation. The results have shown that the unconscious presentation of fearful faces leads to underestimation of numerosity compared to happy and neutral faces. Further experiment has shown that the participants could not detect the presence of facial expressions directly. Together, these results indicate that perceived emotion influences numerosity judgments automatically without conscious awareness, which provides novel evidence linking emotion and quantity perception.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

References

  • Baker JM, Rodzon KS, Jordan K (2013) The impact of emotion on numerosity estimation. Front Psychol 4:Article 521

  • Carlson JM, Reinke KS (2010) Spatial attention-related modulation of the N170 by backward masked fearful faces. Brain Cogn 73(1):20–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo Z, Lega C, Boehringer J, Gallucci M, Girelli L, Carbon C-C (2014) Happiness takes you right: the effect of emotional stimuli on line bisection. Cogn Emot 28(2):325–344

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choi J-W, Kim K, Lee J-H (2016) The influence of attention toward facial expressions on size perception. Psychol Res 80(1):69–75

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cordes S, Goldstein A, Heller E (2014) Sets within sets: the influence of set membership on numerical estimates. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40(1):94–105

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull JT, Vidal F, Nazarian B, Macar F (2004) Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science 303(5663):1506–1508

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson RJ (2003) Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: toward a synthesis. Psychophysiology 40(5):655–665

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson RJ, Shackman AJ, Maxwell JS (2004) Asymmetries in face and brain related to emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 8(9):389–391

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 122(3):371–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doi H, Shinohara K (2009) The perceived duration of emotional face is influenced by the gaze direction. Neurosci Lett 457(2):97–100

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dormal V, Seron X, Pesenti M (2006) Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. Acta Psychol 121(2):109–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dormal V, Dormal G, Joassin F, Pesenti M (2012) A common right fronto-parietal network for numerosity and duration processing: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 33(6):1490–1501

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Droit-Volet S, Mermillod M, Cocenas-Silva R, Gil S (2010) The effect of expectancy of a threatening event on time perception in human adults. Emotion 10(6):908–914

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egner T, Etkin A, Gale S, Hirsch J (2008) Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cereb Cortex 18(6):1475–1484

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esteves F, Parra C, Dimberg U, Ohman A (1994) Nonconscious associative learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli. Psychophysiology 31(4):375–385

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 14(3):340–347

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI (2005) The activation of attentional networks. NeuroImage 26(2):471–479

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson L, Dehaene S, Spelke E (2004) Core systems of number. Trends Cogn Sci 8(7):307–314

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gil S, Droit-Volet S (2012) Emotional time distortions: the fundamental role of arousal. Cogn Emot 26(5):847–862

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haase SJ, Fisk GD (2015) Awareness of “invisible” arrows in a metacontrast masking paradigm. Am J Psychol 128(1):15–30

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi MJ, Kanai R, Tanabe HC, Yoshida Y, Carlson S, Walsh V, Sadato N (2013) Interaction of numerosity and time in prefrontal and parietal cortex. J Neurosci 33(3):883–893

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes KJ, Lourenco SF (2011) Common spatial organization of number and emotional expression: a mental magnitude line. Brain Cogn 77(2):315–323

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard VR, Sann C, Spelke ES, Streri A (2009) Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(25):10382–10385

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Javadi AH, Aichelburg C (2012) When time and numerosity interfere: the longer the more, and the more the longer. PLoS One 7(7):e41496

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jewell G, McCourt ME (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38(1):93–110

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan KE, Brannon EM (2006) A common representational system governed by Weber’s law: nonverbal numerical similarity judgments in 6-year-olds and rhesus macaques. J Exp Child Psychol 95(3):215–229

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster EHW, Crombez G, Verschuere B, De Houwer J (2004) Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behav Res Ther 42(10):1183–1192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux J (2012) Rethinking the Emotional Brain. Neuron 73(4):653–676

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liu P, Yang W, Yuan X, Bi C, Chen A, Huang X (2015) Individual alerting efficiency modulates time perception. Front Psychol 6:386

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher T, Brugger P (2007) Exploring number space by random digit generation. Exp Brain Res 180(4):655–665

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher T, Schwarz U, Schubiger M, Brugger P (2008) Head turns bias the brain’s internal random generator. Curr Biol 18(2):R60–R62

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luck SJ, Hillyard SA (1994) Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20(5):1000–1014

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A (1998) The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces-KDEF. CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet. ISBN 91-630-7164-9

  • Macmillan NA, Kaplan HL (1985) Detection theory analysis of group data. estimating sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. Psychol Bull 98(1):185–199

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg K, Bradley BP (2002) Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behav Res Ther 40(12):1403–1414

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman A, Carlsson K, Lundqvist D, Ingvar M (2007) On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiol Behav 92(1–2):180–185

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips LT, Weisbuch M, Ambady N (2014) People perception: social vision of groups and consequences for organizing and interacting. Res Org Behav 34:101–127

    Google Scholar 

  • Raz A, Buhle (2006) Typologies of attentional networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 7(5):367–379

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz PA, Kinsbourne M, Moscovitch M (1990) Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain Cogn 12(2):240–266

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross J, Burr DC (2010) Vision senses number directly. J Vis 10(2):1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth HL, Lora AN, Heilman KM (2002) Effects of monocular viewing and eye dominance on spatial attention. Brain 125(9):2023–2035

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schirmer A (2011a) How emotions change time. Front Integr Neurosci 5:58

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schirmer A (2011b) How emotions change time. Front Integr Neurosci 5:58

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shimizu H, Imae K (1981) Nihongoban state-trait anxiety inventory no sakusei. Jpn J Educ Psychol 29:348–353 (in Japanese)

  • Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Luchene RE (1970) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (self-evaluation questionnaire). Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan J, Ma Z, Gao X, Wu Y, Fang F (2011) Gender difference of unconscious attentional bias in high trait anxiety individuals. PLoS One 6(5):e20305

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tortella-Feliu M, Morillas-Romero A, Balle M, Bornas X, Llabrés J, Pacheco-Unguetti AP (2014) Attentional control, attentional network functioning, and emotion regulation styles. Cogn Emot 28(5):769–780

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsuchiya N, Koch C (2005) Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nat Neurosci 8(8):1096–1101

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallesi A, Binns MA, Shallice T (2008) An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition 107(2):501–527

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Schalk J, Hawk ST, Fischer AH, Doosje BJ (2011) Moving faces, looking places: the Amsterdam dynamic facial expressions set (ADFES). Emotion 11:907–920

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vetter P, Butterworth B, Bahrami B (2008) Modulating attentional load affects numerosity estimation: evidence against a pre-attentive subitizing mechanism. PLoS One 3(9):e3269

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vicario CM, Pecoraro P, Turriziani P, Koch G, Caltagirone C, Oliveri M (2008) Relativistic compression and expansion of experiential time in the left and right space. PLoS One 3(3):e1716

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani R (2013) Emotion regulation, attention to emotion, and the ventral attentional network. Front Hum Neurosci 7:746

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vos PG, van Oeffelen MP, Tibosch HJ, Allik J (1988) Interactions between area and numerosity. Psychol Res 50(3):148–154

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7(11):483–488

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willenbockel V, Sadr J, Fiset D, Horne GO, Gosselin F, Tanaka JW (2010) Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behav Res Methods 42(3):671–684

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams LM, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, Bryant RA, Meares RA, Peduto AS, Gordon E (2006) Amygdala–prefrontal dissociation of subliminal and supraliminal fear. Hum Brain Mapp 27(8):652–661

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu F, Spelke ES (2000) Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition 74(1):B1–B11

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamada Y, Kawabe T (2011) Emotion colors time perception unconsciously. Conscious Cogn 20(4):1835–1841

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young LN, Cordes S (2013) Fewer things, lasting longer: the effects of emotion on quantity judgments. Psychol Sci 24(6):1057–1059

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was partly supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 24791218 to HD.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kazuyuki Shinohara.

Additional information

Handling Editor: Martin H. Fischer (University of Potsdam).

Reviewers: Kevin Holmes (Colorado College), Matthias Hartmann (University of Bern).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Doi, H., Shinohara, K. Emotional faces influence numerosity estimation without awareness. Cogn Process 17, 389–397 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0774-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0774-5

Keywords

Navigation