Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring number space by random digit generation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is some evidence that human subjects preferentially select small numbers when asked to sample numbers from large intervals “at random”. A retrospective analysis of single digit frequencies in 16 independent experiments with the Mental Dice Task (generation of digits 1–6 during 1 min) confirmed the occurrence of small-number biases (SNBs) in 488 healthy subjects. A subset of these experiments suggested a spatial nature of this bias in the sense of a “leftward” shift along the number line. First, individual SNBs were correlated with leftward deviations in a number line bisection task (but unrelated to the bisection of physical lines). Second, in 20 men, the magnitude of SNBs significantly correlated with leftward attentional biases in the judgment of chimeric faces. Finally, cognitive activation of the right hemisphere enhanced SNBs in 20 different men, while left hemisphere activation reduced them. Together, these findings provide support for a spatial component in random number generation. Specifically, they allow an interpretation of SNBs in terms of “pseudoneglect in number space.” We recommend the use of random digit generation for future explorations of spatial-attentional asymmetries in numerical processing and discuss methodological issues relevant to prospective designs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Abbreviations

RNG:

Random number generation

MDT:

Mental dice task

SNB:

Small-number bias

LB:

Line bisection

LVF:

Left visual field

RVF:

Right visual field

References

  • Baird JC, Noma E (1975) Psychophysical study of numbers I. Generation of numerical responses. Psychol Res Psychol Forsch 37:281–297

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banks WP, Coleman MJ (1981) Two subjective scales of number. Percept Psychophys 29:95–105

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Banks WP, Hill DK (1974) Apparent magnitude of number scaled by random production. J Exp Psychol 102:353–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borod JC, Haywood CS, Koff E (1997) Neuropsychological aspects of facial asymmetry during emotional expression: a review of the normal adult literature. Neuropsychol Rev 7:41–60

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers D, Heilman KM (1980) Pseudoneglect—effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18:491–498

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger P (1997) Variables that influence the generation of random sequences: an update. Percept Mot Skills 84:627–661

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger P, Milicevic A, Regard M, Cook ND (1993) Random-number generation and the menstrual-cycle—preliminary evidence for a premenstrual alteration of frontal-lobe functioning. Percept Mot Skills 77:915–921

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger P, Monsch AU, Salmon DP, Butters N (1996) Random number generation in dementia of the Alzheimer type: a test of frontal executive functions. Neuropsychologia 34:97–103

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger P, Pietzsch S, Weidmann G, Biro P, Alon E (1995) Stroop-type interference in random-number generation. Psychol Rep 77:387–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Bub D, Gum T (1990) Psychlab. In: McGill Technical Manuals, McGill University, Montreal

  • Bull R, Benson PJ (2006) Digit ratio (2D: 4D) and the spatial representation of magnitude. Horm Behav 50:194–199

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bultitude JH, Aimola Davies AM (2006) Putting attention on the line: investigating the activation-orientation hypothesis of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia 44:1849–1858

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill L (2006) Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:477–484

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Calabria M, Rossetti Y (2005) Interference between number processing and line bisection: a methodology. Neuropsychologia 43:779–783

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell JJ (2000) Neuropsychiatric assessment. In: Coffey CE, Cummings JL (eds) Textbook of Geriatric Neuropsychiatry. American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington, DC, pp 109–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Casarotti M, Michielin M, Zorzi M, Umilta C (2007) Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition 102:101–117

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels C, Witt K, Wolff S, Jansen O, Deuschl G (2003) Rate dependency of the human cortical network subserving executive functions during generation of random number series—a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett 345:25–28

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • de Hevia MD, Girelli L, Vallar G (2006) Numbers and space: a cognitive illusion? Exp Brain Res 168:254–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S (1997) The number sense. How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen 122:371–396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Mehler J (1992) Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words. Cognition 43:1–29

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L (2003) Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol 20:487–506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diekamp B, Regolin L, Gunturkun O, Vallortigara G (2005) A left-sided visuospatial bias in birds. Curr Biol 15:R372–R373

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Doricchi F, Guariglia P, Gasparini M, Tomaiuolo F (2005) Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nat Neurosci 8:1663–1665

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fias W, Fischer MH (2005) Spatial representation of numbers. In: Campbell JID (eds) Handbook of mathematical cognition. Psychology Press, New York, pp 43–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH (2001) Number processing induces spatial performance biases. Neurology 57:822–826

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH (2003) Spatial representations in number processing—evidence from a pointing task. Vis Cogn 10:493–508

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH, Castel AD, Dodd MD, Pratt J (2003) Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 6:555–556

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH, Warlop N, Hill RL, Fias W (2004) Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Exp Psychol 51:91–97

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gitelman DR, Nobre AC, Parrish TB, LaBar KS, Kim YH, Meyer JR, Mesulam M (1999) A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. Brain 122:1093–1106

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gobel SM, Calabria M, Farne A, Rossetti Y (2006) Parietal rTMS distorts the mental number line: Simulating “spatial” neglect in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia 44:860–868

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S (2005) Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:435–448

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jahanshahi M, Dirnberger G, Fuller R, Frith CD (2000) The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: a study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 12:713–725

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jewell G, McCourt ME (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38:93–110

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne M, Cook J (1971) Generalized and lateralized effects of concurrent verbalization on a unimanual skill. Q J Exp Psychol 23:341–345

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoch D, Brugger P, Regard M (2005) Suppressing versus releasing a habit: frequency-dependent effects of prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex 15:885–887

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee GP, Struss E, Loring DW, McCloskey L, Haworth JM, Lehman RAW (1997) Sensitivity of figural fluency on the five-point test to focal neurological dysfunction. Clin Neuropsychol 11:59–68

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Luh KE, Gooding DC (1999) Perceptual biases in psychosis-prone individuals. J Abnorm Psychol 108:283–289

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Luh KE, Rueckert LM, Levy J (1991) Perceptual asymmetries for free viewing of several types of chimeric stimuli. Brain Cogn 16:83–103

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt ME, Jewell G (1999) Visuospatial attention in line bisection: stimulus modulation of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia 37:843–855

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer RS, Landauer TK (1967) Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature 215:1519–1520

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls MER, Roberts GR (2002) Can free-viewing perceptual asymmetries be explained by scanning, pre-motor or attentional biases? Cortex 38:113–136

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noma E, Baird JC (1975) Psychophysical study of numbers II. Theoretical models of number generation. Psychol Res Psychol Forsch 38:81–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk HC, Wood G, Willmes K (2005) The universal SNARC effect—the association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Exp Psychol 52:187–194

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri M, Rausei V, Koch G, Torriero S, Turriziani P, Caltagirone C (2004) Overestimation of numerical distances in the left side of space. Neurology 63:2139–2141

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Priftis K, Zorzi M, Meneghello F, Marenzi R, Umilta C (2006) Explicit versus implicit processing of representational space in neglect: dissociations in accessing the mental number line. J Cogn Neurosci 18:680–688

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rath G (1966) Randomization by humans. Am J Psychol 79:97–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regard M, Strauss E, Knapp P (1982) Children’s production on verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks. Percept Mot Skills 55:839–844

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Restle F (1970) Speed of adding and comparing numbers. J Exp Psychol 83:274–278

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz PA, Kinsbourne M, Moscovitch M (1990) Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain Cogn 12:240–266

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti Y, Jacquin-Courtois S, Rode G, Ota H, Michel C, Boisson D (2004) Does action make the link between number and space representation? Visuo-manual adaptation improves number bisection in unilateral neglect. Psychol Sci 15:426–430

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rueckert L, Deravanesian A, Baboorian D, Lacalamita A, Repplinger M (2002) Pseudoneglect and the cross-over effect. Neuropsychologia 40:162–173

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schueli H, Henn V, Brugger P (1999) Vestibular stimulation affects dichotic lexical decision performance. Neuropsychologia 37:653–659

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schütze H, Bürki P, Brugger P, Koller E (1993) Neuropsychological reactions at moderate altitude. In: Sutton J, Houston C, Coates G (eds) Hypoxia and molecular medicine. Queen City Printers Inc., Burlington, p 313

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz W, Keus IM (2004) Moving the eyes along the mental number line: comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Percept Psychophys 66:651–664

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seron X, Pesenti M, Noel MP, Deloche G, Cornet JA (1992) Images of numbers, or when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue. Cognition 44:159–196

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shuren J, Hartley T, Heilman KM (1998) The effects of rotation on spatial attention. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 11:72–75

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tranel D, Damasio H, Denburg NL, Bechara A (2005) Does gender play a role in functional asymmetry of ventromedial prefrontal cortex? Brain 128:2872–2881

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier P, Ortigue S, Brugger P (2004) The number space and neglect. Cortex 40:399–410

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi M, Priftis K, Meneghello F, Marenzi R, Umilta C (2006) The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia 44:1061–1067

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi M, Priftis K, Umilta C (2002) Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature 417:138–139

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the investigators of the original experiments on RNG for having administered the Mental Dice Task. We also acknowledge financial support by the Betty and David Koetser foundation (to PB).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tobias Loetscher.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Loetscher, T., Brugger, P. Exploring number space by random digit generation. Exp Brain Res 180, 655–665 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0889-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-0889-0

Keywords

Navigation