Introduction

Play is a characteristic behavior of mammals, occurring in young of nearly all mammalian species (Fagen 1981; Bekoff and Byers 1998; Burghardt 2005). Play behavior is generally categorized as social play, involving interactions with conspecifics, or non-social play, involving locomotion or manipulation of objects (Burghardt 2011). Various adaptive functions have been proposed for play behavior in young animals including the promotion of motor, cognitive, and social development, and acquisition of behavioral plasticity in responses to unfamiliar situations (Bekoff 1972, 1988; Poirier and Smith 1974; Bekoff and Byers 1998; Lewis 2000; Špinka et al. 2001). The expression of play behavior may be sexually dimorphic within species, suggesting in some cases that play may have different functions for males and females (Olioff and Stewart 1978; Meder 1990; Pedersen et al. 1990; Pellis et al. 1996; Nunes et al. 1999; Maestripieri and Ross 2004; Paukner and Suomi 2008; Auger and Olesen 2009). Play behavior is associated with improved motor skill and coordination during the juvenile period in Belding’s ground squirrels (Urocitellus beldingi; Nunes et al. 2004a, b). Here I evaluated possible benefits of play behavior beyond the juvenile period in female U. beldingi.

Play behavior is related to a variety of benefits to young animals while they are juveniles. For example, higher rates of play among young horses (Equus caballus) are associated with enhanced body condition and greater survival during the juvenile period (Cameron et al. 2008). Play behavior also improves juvenile survival in brown bears (Ursus arctos; Fagen and Fagen 2004). Play-related improvements in motor skill and coordination in juvenile male U. beldingi are associated with early dispersal from the natal area (Nunes et al. 2004a). In red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) social play facilitates the establishment and maintenance of dominance relationships among juveniles (Meyer and Weber 1996), and in common seals (Phoca vitulina) social play behavior helps young animals become integrated into their social groups (Wilson 1974). Moreover, Thompson (1996) suggested that social play in sable antelope (Hippotragus niger) allows young animals to assess their physical and behavioral development relative to that of their peers.

Adaptive benefits of play behavior to individuals beyond the juvenile period are less clear than benefits during early development. However, some studies suggest that the benefits of juvenile play may extend into adulthood. Juvenile play behavior improves long-term survival in U. arctos (Fagen and Fagen 2009). Moreover, social play during the juvenile period has been proposed to influence adult social interactions and behavior. For example, it has been suggested that features of juvenile social play reflect adult social roles (Maestripieri and Ross 2004), and that juvenile social play helps forge social relationships that will be important later in life (Rho et al. 2007; Ward et al. 2008). Blumstein et al. (2013) observed that in yellow-bellied marmots (Marmota flaviventris) dominance relationships established during play interactions early in life persist into adulthood. It has also been suggested that juvenile social play functions to refine motor skills and behaviors that are important in adulthood. Social play is often similar to adult aggressive and mating behaviors (e.g., Hass and Jenni 1993; Nunes et al. 1999; Pasztor et al. 2001), and has been hypothesized to serve as practice for these behaviors or promote their neurobehavioral maturation (Caro 1988; Byers and Walker 1995). For example, in rats (Rattus norvegicus) juvenile social play has a critical role in shaping adult expression of agonistic and sexual behaviors and emotional responses associated with these behaviors (Hol et al. 1999; van den Berg et al. 1999).

Here I evaluated whether juvenile social play in female U. beldingi is associated with the later expression of behaviors important in defending a maternal territory. Infanticide by conspecifics is common in mammalian species, including U. beldingi, and female mammals typically have strategies to defend young against this threat (Hrdy 1979; Sherman 1981b). Female U. beldingi establish maternal territories during gestation and defend the territories throughout lactation, aggressively evicting intruders who wander onto a territory (Nunes et al. 2000). I observed the social play behavior of female U. beldingi as juveniles, and later observed maternal territorial behaviors of the same females as yearlings. I evaluated whether rates and intensities of aggressive and vigilant behavior associated with defending territories were correlated with juvenile play.

I further evaluated whether juvenile social play is related to the likelihood of yearling female U. beldingi successfully weaning a litter. Female U. beldingi may begin reproducing as yearlings; however, on average 35 % of yearling females do not successfully wean a litter (Sherman and Morton 1984). I also evaluated whether body mass at the beginning of the yearling reproductive cycle influenced the chances of successfully reproducing. Reproduction, and in particular lactation, is energetically expensive for female mammals (Kenagy et al. 1989). Some juvenile U. beldingi, especially those born later in the active season, fall short of achieving mature body size before entering hibernation (Morton et al. 1974). Thus, as yearlings, females may need to channel energy toward growth, limiting the amount of energy available for reproduction. Play has been shown to be associated with heightened body condition in young animals (Cameron et al. 2008), and hence might influence yearling reproductive success via effects on physical condition and body mass. Finally, I assessed whether having a surviving mother influences reproductive success in yearling female U. beldingi. Closely related female U. beldingi act cooperatively in defending maternal territories (Sherman 1981a), and experience raising young is associated with increased intensity of aggressive and vigilant behavior in defending territories (Nunes 2014). Thus, having assistance from an experienced mother in defending a territory might enhance overall protection of young and improve the chances of successfully weaning a litter in yearling female U. beldingi.

Materials and methods

From May through August 2001–2006 and May through July 2012–2013 a population of U. beldingi was studied in a 50-ha meadow near Tioga Pass in Mono County, CA, USA (37°55′N, 119°15′W). This species is diurnal and inhabits alpine and subalpine meadows in the western United States. Squirrels hibernate 8–9 months each year from late summer through spring. Males do not begin breeding until they are 2 years old, whereas females may begin breeding as yearlings (Jenkens and Eshelman 1984). Females typically enter estrus within a week after emerging from hibernation and mate on only 1 day per year, thus bearing at most one litter per year (Morton and Gallup 1975). Young remain underground in natal burrows during lactation and first emerge from the natal burrow when they are about 25–28 days old, near the time of weaning (Holekamp et al. 1984). Reproductive females maintain maternal territories during gestation, lactation, and the period in which young first emerge from the natal burrow (Nunes et al. 2000). Juveniles engage in play behavior during their first 2 weeks above ground, after which rates of play decline substantially as juveniles roam farther from the natal burrow. Most play interactions (> 97 %) occur between littermates (Nunes et al. 1999). All surviving males emigrate from the natal area before reproducing, whereas females tend to be philopatric and remain in their natal areas throughout their lives (Holekamp 1984).

Squirrels were captured in live-traps (Tomahawk Live-Trap Company, Hazelhurst, WI, USA) baited with peanut butter. Traps were checked every 30 min or less during trapping sessions. At their first capture, squirrels were fitted with monel metal ear tags (National Band and Tag Co., Newport, KY, USA) for permanent identification. The fur of squirrels was marked with unique symbols using hair dye (Clairol, Stamford, CT, USA) to aid in visual identification of individuals during behavioral observations. Body mass of squirrels was measured with spring balance scales (Avinet, Dryden, NY, USA). Females were observed daily throughout the day (0730–1800 h) from elevated posts such as boulders or hilltops to locate their maternal territories. Behavioral cues used to identify maternal territories included excavation of burrows, carrying of nest material into burrows, and initiation of aggressive interactions with conspecifics (Nunes et al. 1997, 2000). Maternal burrows of reproductive females were identified as the burrow that a female first emerged from in the morning or last entered in the evening. After their location was determined, maternal territories of lactating females were observed daily to determine the dates on which their young first emerged from the maternal burrow. Young were trapped within 2 days of their first appearance above ground during which time they remain close to the natal burrow and can be unambiguously assigned to mothers (Holekamp 1984). Adult females in the study were classified as multiparous if they had successfully weaned a litter in an earlier year. Distances between maternal burrows of reproductive females were determined using aerial photographs obtained from the United States Geological Survey. All work with U. beldingi in this study followed humane guidelines published for mammals (Sikes et al. 2011).

Behavioral observations

The behavior of juvenile U. beldingi from 87 litters was observed during the summers of 2001, 2002, 2004, and 2012. A total of 81 females from these litters were included in the study as yearlings. The juvenile behavior of these 81 females was observed for an average of 537.7 ± 29.1 min over an average of 8.0 ± 0.4 days. Behavioral observations of juveniles were conducted throughout the day between 0700 and 1800 h from elevated posts such as boulders or hilltops. Juveniles were observed during the 2-week period following their first emergence from the natal burrow. During observations, all occurrences of social play behavior were recorded. Rates of social play were calculated for individuals as the number of social play interactions per hour of observation. Specific play behaviors are based on descriptions by Nunes et al. (1999) and are described below.

Social play behavior

  • Wrestling the juvenile faces a partner, typically in a ventrum to ventrum clench or embrace, and pecks at the partner’s neck, throat, cheeks, or abdomen without inflicting bite wounds.

  • Tackling the juvenile jumps or pounces on a partner, usually from a running start.

  • Boxing the juvenile bats with the forepaws at a partner, who usually reciprocates.

  • Chasing the juvenile follows and pursues a partner while both juveniles are running.

  • Mounting and play copulation the juvenile climbs on the back of a partner and places the forepaws around the partner’s chest or abdomen, grasping in the mouth the skin of the partner’s neck, cheek, or back, and aligning the pelvis with the partner’s pelvis; the juvenile and partner typically both lie on their sides on the ground; no intromission occurs.

During the summers of 2002–2006, the behavior of reproductive females was observed during gestation, lactation, and the week-long period after emergence from the natal burrow in a total sample of 82 adult and 32 yearling females who weaned litters. Females were observed for an average of 149.8 ± 5.9 min during gestation, 141.3 ± 5.2 min during lactation, and 88.4 ± 4.8 min during the post-emergence period. Behavioral surveys were conducted between 0700 and 1830 h from elevated posts such as rocks or hilltops. During behavioral surveys, all occurrences of aggressive and vigilant behavior were recorded. Vigilant behaviors included actions with the potential to gather information for the assessment of risks posed by conspecifics, predators, or other threats. Aggressive behaviors included actions with the potential to evict conspecifics or predators from maternal territories. Specific behaviors are based on prior descriptions of behavior in U. beldingi (Nunes et al. 2000) and are described below.

Aggressive behavior

  • Threat the squirrel orients toward or rapidly approaches another squirrel, chatters its teeth, holds its tail upright, and twitches its tail with the fur erect.

  • Chase the squirrel approaches another squirrel in a threat position and runs after it when it moves away.

  • Attack the squirrel approaches another squirrel in a threat position, pounces or lunges at it and inflicts a bite wound on its rump, flank, chest, neck, or head.

  • Fight the squirrel faces another squirrel, usually in a ventrum to ventrum clench, and inflicts bite wounds on the chest, neck, or cheeks of the other squirrel.

Vigilant behavior

  • Rest alert the squirrel is positioned with all four legs on the ground and its head elevated; the squirrel’s body may also be elevated.

  • Post the squirrel stands on its hind legs with its back erect or semi-erect and forelimbs off the ground.

At least three surveys were conducted for each female during gestation, at least three during lactation, and at least two during the post-emergence period. Focal animals were observed for 30–60 min during each survey. Surveys were occasionally terminated early when females wandered out of site or retreated down burrows. Rates of behavior were calculated for individuals as the number of occurrences of behavior per hour of observation.

To estimate the intensity of aggressive and vigilant behavior, I assigned scores to specific behaviors, with values increasing as the intensity level of behavior increased (Nunes 2014). The intensity of aggressive behavior was considered to increase as it increased the likelihood that a conspecific or predator such as a weasel would be evicted from the maternal territory. Intensity scores for aggressive behavior were assigned as follows: threat, 1; short chase (≤ 5 m), 2; intermediate chase (> 5 m but ≤ 15 m), 3; long chase (> 15 m but ≤ 30 m), 4; very long chase (> 30 m), 5; attack or fight, 5. The intensity of vigilant behavior was considered to increase as it enhanced the potential for assessing risk. Intensity scores for vigilant behavior were assigned as follows: resting alert, 1; posting, 3. An additional point was added to a vigilance score if the behavior occurred on an elevated post such as a rock or fallen tree.

Statistical analysis

Variables evaluated in the study included body mass, social play behavior of juvenile females, and rates and intensities of vigilant and aggressive behavior of yearling and adult females. Body mass data followed the normal distribution (Lilliefors test, P > 0.05), but behavioral data did not and so were log transformed to values that were normally distributed (transformed value = ln[original value]). Multiple linear regression with forward stepping was used to evaluate associations between juvenile social play behavior and rates and intensities of vigilant and aggressive behavior among reproductive yearling females. Discriminant analysis with forward stepping was used to evaluate whether juvenile social play behavior and body mass early in the yearling reproductive period were reliable predictors of whether a yearling female would successfully wean a litter. I used χ 2 tests to assess whether yearling females who had a surviving mother were more likely to wean a litter than yearling females who did not. Discriminant analysis with forward stepping was used to evaluate associations between rates and intensities of aggressive behavior in adult females and whether they had a surviving yearling daughter. Independent t tests were used to compare body mass and behavior between specific groups of females. Assumptions of homoscedasticity were met in t-tests (Levene’s test, P ≥ 0.05). Independent variables in multiple regression and discriminant analysis were checked for multicollinearity with Pearson’s r. Statistical tests were performed with Systat 13 (Systat Software, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). For ease of interpretation, values of variables prior to transformation are presented in figures. Mean values are presented ± 1 SEM. Probabilities were considered significant when P ≤ 0.05.

Results

I used multiple linear regression to evaluate the relationship between rates of social play as a juvenile and rates and intensity of vigilant and aggressive behavior during gestation, lactation, and emergence of young from the natal burrow among yearling female U. beldingi who successfully weaned a litter. The independent variables in this analysis did not show multicollinearity (−0.26 < r < 0.37, n = 96). Rates of neither aggressive nor vigilant behavior as a yearling mother were related to juvenile social play. However, there was a significant association between juvenile social play and the intensity of aggressive (Fig. 1a, R 2 = 0.14, F 1,30 = 4.77, P = 0.037) and vigilant behavior (Fig. 1b, R 2 = 0.24, F 1,30 = 9.62, P = 0.004) during gestation in yearling females, but not during other phases of the reproductive period. The intensity of both vigilance and aggression during gestation as a yearling tended to increase with rates of social play as a juvenile (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Association between social play behavior as a juvenile and the intensity of a aggressive and b vigilant behavior during gestation among reproductive female U. beldingi during the yearling active period. The sample included 32 females

I also evaluated whether success in weaning a litter among yearling female U. beldingi was associated with rates of juvenile social play behavior, body mass at the beginning of the reproductive period, or having a surviving mother. Discriminant analysis indicated that both rates of juvenile social play (F 1,79 = 14.08, P < 0.001) and body mass at the beginning of the reproductive period (F 1,78 = 15.63, P < 0.001) were reliable predictors of whether female U. beldingi weaned a litter as a yearling. Yearling females who weaned a litter had higher rates of social play as a juvenile (Fig. 2a, t 79 = 3.75, P < 0.001) and higher body mass at the beginning of the reproductive period (Fig. 2b, t 79 = 3.79, P < 0.001) than did yearling females who were unsuccessful in weaning a litter. I did not observe a significant correlation between rates of juvenile social play and body mass at the beginning of the yearling reproductive period (r = 0.06, n = 81). Rates of social play as a juvenile did not differ between yearling females who did and did not have a surviving mother.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Comparison between yearling female U. beldingi who did and did not wean a litter in a rates of social play as a juvenile, b body mass at the beginning of the reproductive period, and c whether the female’s mother was still alive. Asterisks indicate significant differences between groups of females. The sample included 39 yearling females who weaned a litter and 42 who did not

Having a surviving mother was also associated with increased likelihood of weaning a litter. A total of 64.1 % (25/39) of yearling females in the study whose mother was alive weaned a litter, whereas only 21.4 % (9/42) of yearling females whose mother was not alive weaned a litter (Fig. 2c, χ 21  = 15.12, P < 0.001). The maternal territories of yearling females tended to be near those of their surviving mothers, with the mean distance between the maternal burrows of yearling females and their mothers being 18.3 ± 2.3 m (n = 32).

I assessed whether the behavior of adult females across the reproductive period was associated with whether they had a surviving yearling daughter. Only adult females who weaned a litter during the current summer were included in this analysis, and adult females with a surviving yearling daughter were included only if their daughter also weaned a litter. Because prior experience raising young can influence aggressive and vigilant behavior in maternal U. beldingi (Nunes 2014), only multiparous adult females were included in this analysis. Behavioral variables in this analysis did not exhibit multicollinearity (0.01 < r < 0.38, n = 82). Discriminant analysis indicated that rates of aggressive behavior among adult female U. beldingi were significantly associated with whether they had a reproductive yearling daughter (F 1,80 = 5.55, P = 0.021), but rates of vigilant behavior and the intensity of aggressive and vigilant behavior were not. In particular, adult females with a yearling daughter had significantly greater rates of aggressive behavior than those who did not (Fig. 3, t 80 = 2.36, P = 0.021). The mean distance between the maternal burrows of adult females and their yearling daughters in this analysis was 16.3 ± 1.8 m (n = 20). All 20 females with a reproductive yearling daughter were observed to initiate aggressive interactions toward conspecifics within 5 m of the daughter’s maternal burrow, and 15.9 % (48/301) of aggressive interactions initiated by these females were initiated within 5 m of the daughter’s maternal burrow.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Aggressive behavior during the reproductive period of adult female U. beldingi with and without a reproductive yearling daughter nearby. The asterisk indicates a significant difference between groups of females. The sample included 20 adult females with a yearling daughter and 62 adult females without a yearling daughter

Discussion

I observed a correlation between juvenile social play behavior in female U. beldingi and the intensity of territorial aggressive and vigilant behaviors during gestation in the yearling reproductive period. Females who engaged in social play at higher rates as juveniles also tended to express greater intensity of aggressive and vigilant behavior early in the yearling reproductive cycle. This result is consistent with the idea that play behavior provides training for maternal territorial behavior in young females. However, correlations between juvenile play and yearling behavior did not persist beyond gestation in the yearling reproductive cycle, suggesting that experience may also refine the expression of maternal territorial behavior. Nunes (2014) observed that experience raising young is associated with increased intensity of maternal territorial behaviors as well as greater accuracy in assessing potential threats on maternal territories.

The association between juvenile play and yearling behavior in this study was somewhat limited. Some studies have failed to indicate distinct associations between juvenile play and later behavior. For example, in meerkats (Suricata suricatta) the social play of young animals was not related to the expression of aggressive behavior or success in winning fights later in life (Sharpe and Cherry 2003; Sharpe 2005). Caro (1988) noted that in various species juvenile play typically comprises a small portion of a young animal’s time and energy budgets, and suggested that in some cases juvenile play might be associated with relatively low costs and small improvements or subtle refinements in adult behavior. However, in some species the costs associated with play behavior are substantial, suggesting that the long-term benefits or consequences of play might also be substantial. For example, peaks in social play and social connectivity coincide with peaks in disease transmission and mortality in young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and ontogenetic changes in play behavior have been suggested to underlie predictable cycles of infant and juvenile mortality in this species (Kuehl et al. 2008). Overall, the functional consequences of play during and beyond the juvenile period as well as the costs of play may vary broadly among species, reflecting divergent evolutionary pathways across taxa (Burghardt 2005; Pellis and Pellis 2009).

Although the relationship between juvenile play and yearling behavior in this study may have been limited, it appears to have influenced reproductive success in yearling female U. beldingi. Rates of juvenile social play were higher among yearling females who successfully weaned a litter compared to those who did not. Greater body mass at the beginning of the active period was also associated with a higher likelihood of successfully weaning a litter among yearling females. Thus, juvenile social play may have influenced yearling reproductive success via effects on body mass and body condition rather than effects on yearling behavior. However, juvenile social play was not correlated with body mass of yearling females at the beginning of the active period, so I believe this unlikely.

Greater intensity of aggressive and vigilant behavior during gestation may better equip yearling females to establish a maternal territory. Nunes et al. (1997) observed that female U. beldingi compete for territorial sites at the beginning of the reproductive period and suggested that yearling females might generally be less successful at establishing a maternal territory than are older females. Greater intensity or proficiency in aggressive behavior might confer an advantage to yearling females in establishing a maternal territory and thus successfully weaning a litter. Moreover, reproductive success as a yearling might increase overall lifetime reproductive success. Sherman and Morton (1984) observed that weaning a litter as a yearling does not adversely affect survival or future life expectancy in female U. beldingi.

Although juvenile social play may influence reproductive success in yearling female U. beldingi, other factors may also have a notable impact on reproductive success. Yearling female U. beldingi in this study were more likely to wean a litter if their mother were still alive. Mothers and yearling daughters tended to have maternal territories in close proximity to each other. Mothers with a surviving yearling daughter exhibited elevated overall rates of aggression and directed aggressive behavior to intruders on the daughter’s territory. Sherman (1981a) similarly observed that closely related female U. beldingi often have contiguous territories and assist each other in evicting intruders from the territories.

Associations between juvenile social play and behavior beyond the juvenile period may be related to influences of play on the brain. Byers and Walker (1995) suggested that juvenile play might influence proficiency of adult behavior via effects on late-developing structures involved in motor function, such as the cerebellum. Moreover, social play behavior is associated with synaptic remodeling and increased growth factor expression in specific brain regions (Cooke and Shukla 2011), and social play and peer interaction in juvenile male rats can prevent deficits in adult sexual behavior associated with lesions to the medial preoptic area of the hypothalamus (Twiggs et al. 1978; Leedy et al. 1980). Also in rats, juvenile social play influences dendritic arborization in the medial prefrontal cortex, possibly to promote development of motor responses. Juvenile social play along with other social interactions also influences dendritic arborization in the orbitofrontal cortex, possibly to promote development of abilities to discriminate among social partners and relationships with conspecifics (Bell et al. 2010). Moreover, in rats, juvenile social play increases neural plasticity in the medial prefrontal cortex later in life, suggesting that juvenile play might enhance long-term behavioral flexibility (Himmler et al. 2013). I note, however, that possible relationships between juvenile social play, brain development, and maternal territorial behavior in U. beldingi remain to be elucidated.

Individual differences in boldness and fearfulness are common in animals (Coleman and Wilson 1998; Webster et al. 2009; Bergvall et al. 2011; Dammhahn and Almeling 2012). It is possible that correlations between juvenile social play and yearling aggressive and vigilant behavior in U. beldingi are related to innate differences in boldness. Individuals who are especially bold might engage in juvenile play at high rates and also exhibit higher intensity in aggressive and vigilant behaviors as yearlings. However, I believe this unlikely. Correlations between juvenile play and yearling behavior in female U. beldingi were present only during gestation. If boldness were responsible for these correlations, they would be expected to persist across the entire reproductive period. Alternatively, social interactions in juvenile play might mediate increases in boldness among juveniles that persist to the beginning of the yearling active period, resulting in greater intensity in aggressive and vigilant territorial behavior early in the yearly reproductive cycle. Maternal experience might further shape aggressive and territorial behavior as the reproductive period progresses (Nunes 2014). Greater boldness is associated with higher degrees of aggression toward conspecific intruders in some animals (e.g., Carere et al. 2005), and social interactions with peers can modify boldness and fearfulness in individuals (e.g., Frost et al. 2007). However, the specific relationship between juvenile social play and boldness in U. beldingi remains to be determined.

Overall, results of this study indicate that juvenile social play in female U. beldingi is related to maternal territorial behavior and reproductive success as a yearling, and are consistent with the idea that juvenile social play behavior has benefits beyond the juvenile period in U. beldingi.