Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Now you make false memories; now you do not: the order of presentation of words in DRM lists influences the production of the critical lure in Alzheimer’s disease

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Why do some Alzheimer’s patients produce fewer false memories than healthy older participants in the DeeseRoedigerMcDermott paradigm, which was especially designed for the study of false memories in a laboratory setting? Using a very simple methodology, this study examines a new explanatory factor inherent in the paradigm itself: the order of presentation of the words in the lists. A sample comprising 149 participants (36 younger, 40 middle-aged, 37 healthy older adults, and 36 Alzheimer’s patients) performed a DRM task with either a classic descending forward associative strength (FAS) presentation order of the words or an ascending FAS presentation order. The results showed that this simple manipulation influenced the production of false memories in Alzheimer’s patients only. Contrary to the other participants, Alzheimer’s patients produced significantly more critical lures in the ascending FAS condition than in the descending FAS condition. These new data, interpreted in the light of serial position effects, invite a reconsideration of the relevance of the DRM paradigm for comparing the production of false memories in Alzheimer’s patients and healthy older participants.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 2, 89–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working Memory: Looking Back and Looking Forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839. doi:10.1038/nrn1201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485–493. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Duchek, J. M., Adams, D., Roediger, H. L., III, McDermott, K. B., & Yerys, B. E. (1999). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16(3–5), 361–384. doi:10.1080/026432999380834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, P. J., Salmon, D. P., Bondi, M. W., Bui, B. K., Olichney, J., Delis, D. C., & Thal, L. J. (2000). Comparison of the serial position effect in very mild Alzheimer’s disease, mild Alzheimer’s disease, and amnesia associated with electroconvulsive therapy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(3), 290–298. doi:10.1017/s1355617700633040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beaunieux, H., Eustache, F., Busson, P., De La Sayette, V., Viader, F., & Desgranges, B. (2012). Cognitive procedural learning in early Alzheimer’s disease: Impaired processes and compensatory mechanisms. Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(1), 31–42. doi:10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02002.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-David, B. M., Tewari, A., Shakuf, V., & Van Lieshout, P. H. (2014). Stroop effects in Alzheimer’s disease: Selective attention speed of processing, or color-naming? A meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 38(4), 923–938. doi:10.3233/JAD-131244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 164–169. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Wright, R., Reyna, V., & Mojardin, A. (2001). Conjoint recognition and phantom recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(2), 307–327. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.27.2.307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruno, D., Reiss, P. T., Petkova, E., Sidtis, J. J., & Pomara, N. (2013). Decreased recall of primacy words predicts cognitive decline. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(2), 95–103. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Budson, A. E., Daffner, K. R., Desikan, R., & Schacter, D. L. (2000). When false recognition is unopposed by true recognition: gist-based memory distortion in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 14(2), 277–287. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.14.2.277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Budson, A. E., Sullivan, A. L., Mayer, E., Daffner, K. R., Black, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2002). Suppression of false recognition in Alzheimer’s disease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Brain, 125(12), 2750–2765. doi:10.1093/brain/awf277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Canolle, M., Messaoudi, M., Ayoub, B., Descours, I., Bocquet, P., Gely-Nargeot, M.-C., & Touchon, J. (2008). Valeur prototypique des intrusions sémantiques dans la maladie d’Alzheimer. Psychologie & NeuroPsychiatrie du vieillissement, 6(1), 67–79. doi:10.1684/pnv.2008.0115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corson, Y., & Verrier, N. (2007). Emotions and false memories valence or arousal? Psychological Science, 18(3), 208–211. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01874.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I., Byrd, M., & Swanson, J. M. (1987). Patterns of memory loss in three elderly samples. Psychology and Aging, 2(1), 79–86. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.2.1.79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(1), 17–22. doi:10.1037/h0046671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehon, H., & Brédart, S. (2004). False memories: young and older adults think of semantic associates at the same rate, but young adults are more successful at source monitoring. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 191–197. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, N. A., Kim, H., & Cabeza, R. (2007). Effects of aging on true and false memory formation: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 45(14), 3157–3166. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derouesné, C., Poitreneau, J., Hugonot, L., Kalafat, M., Dubois, B., & Laurent, B. (1999). Le Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE): Un outil pratique pour l’évaluation de l’état cognitif des patients par le clinicien version française consensuelle. La Presse Médicale, 28(21), 1141–1148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egli, S. C., Beck, I. R., Berres, M., Foldi, N. S., Monsch, A. U., & Sollberger, M. (2014). Serial position effects are sensitive predictors of conversion from MCI to Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(5), S420–S424. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2013.09.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., Fasotti, L., & Allain, P. (2012). Source monitoring in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 80(2), 185–191. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2012.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evrard, C., Colombel, F., Gilet, A. L., & Corson, Y. (2016). Intact semantic priming of critical lures in Alzheimer’s disease: Implications for false memory. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71(4), 671–674. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbv006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foldi, N. S., Brickman, A. M., Schaefer, L. A., & Knutelska, M. E. (2003). Distinct serial position profiles and neuropsychological measures differentiate late life depression from normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Research, 120(1), 71–84. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00163-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foldi, N. S., Lobosco, J. J., & Schaefer, L. A. (2002). The effect of attentional dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: theoretical and practical implications. Paper presented at the Seminars in speech and language.

  • Gallo, D. A. (2006). Associative illusions of memory: False memory research in DRM and related tasks. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 833–848. doi:10.3758/MC.38.7.833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A., Shahid, K. R., Olson, M. A., Solomon, T. M., Schacter, D. L., & Budson, A. E. (2006). Overdependence on degraded gist memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 20(6), 625–632. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.6.625.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giffard, B., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2005). Semantic memory disorders in Alzheimer’s disease: clues from semantic priming effects. Current Alzheimer Research, 2(4), 425–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giffard, B., Desgranges, B., Nore-Mary, F., Lalevée, C., de la Sayette, V., Pasquier, F., & Eustache, F. (2001). The nature of semantic memory deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 124(8), 1522–1532. doi:10.1093/brain/124.8.1522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giffard, B., Laisney, M., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2015). An exploration of the semantic network in Alzheimer’s disease: Influence of emotion and concreteness of concepts. Cortex, 69, 201–211. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.08.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilet, A.-L., Evrard, C., Colombel, F., Tropée, E., Marie, C., & Corson, Y. (2016). False memories in Alzheimer’s disease: Intact semantic priming but impaired production of critical lures. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbw032.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hester, R. L., Kinsella, G. J., & Ong, B. (2004). Effect of age on forward and backward span tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(04), 475–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howieson, D. B., Mattek, N., Seeyle, A. M., Dodge, H. H., Wasserman, D., Zitzelberger, T., & Jeffrey, K. (2011). Serial position effects in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 292–299. doi:10.1080/13803395.2010.516742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, M. R., Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2015). Serial position functions in general knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1715–1727. doi:10.1037/xlm0000141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Galluccio, L., & Stofer, K. A. (1999). Reducing gist-based false recognition in older adults: encoding and retrieval manipulations. Psychology and Aging, 14(2), 220–237. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.14.2.220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • La Rue, A., Hermann, B., Jones, J. E., Johnson, S., Asthana, S., & Sager, M. A. (2008). Effect of parental family history of Alzheimer’s disease on serial position profiles. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 4(4), 285–290. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2008.03.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laisney, M., Giffard, B., Belliard, S., De La Sayette, V., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2011). When the zebra loses its stripes: Semantic priming in early Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Cortex, 47(1), 35–46. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaVoie, D. J., & Faulkner, K. (2000). Age differences in false recognition using a forced choice paradigm. Experimental Aging Research, 26(4), 367–381. doi:10.1080/036107300750015750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martín, M. E., Sasson, Y., Crivelli, L., Roldán Gerschovich, E., Campos, J. A., Calcagno, M. L., & Allegri, R. F. (2013). Relevance of the serial position effect in the differential diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer-type dementia, and normal ageing. Neurología (English Edition), 28(4), 219–225. doi:10.1016/j.nrleng.2012.04.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, D. P., & Smith, A. D. (2002). The effect of warnings on false memories in young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 30(7), 1065–1077. doi:10.3758/BF03194324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(2), 212–230. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.0012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(5), 1177–1194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R., Jr., Kawas, C. H., & Mayeux, R. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7(3), 263–269. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, A. M., Snider, S. F., Campbell, R. E., & Friedman, R. B. (2015). Phonological short-term memory in logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 71, 183–189. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 482–488. doi:10.1037/h0045106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25(6), 838–848. doi:10.3758/BF03211328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ober, B. A. (2002). RT and non-RT methodology for semantic priming research with Alzheimer’s disease patients: A critical review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(7), 883–911. doi:10.1076/jcen.24.7.883.8384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orru, G., Sampietro, S., Catanzaro, S., Girardi, A., Najjar, M., Giantin, V., & Inelmen, E. (2009). Serial position effect in a free recall task: differences between probable dementia of Alzheimer type (PDAT), vascular (VaD) and mixed etiology dementia (MED). Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 49, 207–210. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2009.09.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, R. C., Smith, G., Kokmen, E., Ivnik, R. J., & Tangalos, E. G. (1992). Memory function in normal aging. Neurology, 42(2), 396. doi:10.1212/WNL.42.2.396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (2011). Dual processes in decision making and developmental neuroscience: A fuzzy-trace model. Developmental Review, 31(2), 180–206. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.004.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Balota, D. A., & Watson, J. M. (2001). Spreading activation and arousal of false memories. The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder, 95–115. doi:10.1037/10394-006.

  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803–814. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spaan, P. E., Raaijmakers, J. G., & Jonker, C. (2003). Alzheimer’s disease versus normal ageing: a review of the efficiency of clinical and experimental memory measures. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(2), 216–233. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.2.216.13638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, W. D., & Raz, N. (1995). Differential effects of aging on memory for content and context: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 10(4), 527–539. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(2), 461. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.461.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., & Sommers, M. S. (2005). Attention to item-specific processing eliminates age effects in false memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(1), 71–86. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldie, B. D., & Kwong See, S. T. (2003). Remembering words never presented: False memory effects in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10(4), 281–297. doi:10.1076/anec.10.4.281.28969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. M., Balota, D. A., & Sergent-Marshall, S. D. (2001). Semantic, phonological, and hybrid veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology, 15(2), 254–267. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2000). Manuel de l’Echelle d’Intelligence de Wechsler pour adultes, 3ème édition [Manual of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale, Third edition]: Paris: ECPA.

  • Wright, R. E. (1982). Adult age similarities in free recall output order and strategies. Journal of Gerontology, 37(1), 76–79. doi:10.1093/geronj/37.1.76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank P. Chapelan, M. Ganemtore, C. Marie, and C. Vilhem for their assistance in recruitment and data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christelle Evrard.

Appendix

Appendix

The five French DRM lists with their approximated English translation and Forward Associative Strengths (CLs are in bold).

L1: Chaussure ( Shoe ), Pied (Foot, 24.58), Marche (Walk, 7.74), Lacet (Shoelace, 4.38), Botte (Boot, 3.03), Chaussette (Sock, 2.36), Chausson (Slipper,2.02), Cuir (Leather, 2.02), Pointure (Size, 2.02), Semelle (Outsole, 1.68), Soulier (Shoe, 1.35), Sabot (Clog, 1.01), Talon (Heel, 1.01), Cirage (Shoe Polish, 0.67), Usure (Wear, 0.67), Verni (Patent Leather, 0.67).

L2: Eglise ( Church ), Clocher (Bell Tower, 11.78), Curé (Priest, 7.41), Religion (Religion, 7.41), Croix (Cross, 3.03), Village (Village, 2.36); Prière (Prayer, 2.02), Cathédrale (Cathedral, 1.68), Cloche (Bell, 1.68), Culte (Worship, 1.68), Prêtre (Minister, 1.68), Messe (Mass, 1.01), Temple (Temple, 1.01), Catholique (Catholic, 0.67), Chapelle (Chapel, 0.67), Monastère (Monastery, 0.67).

L3: Laine ( Wool ), Mouton (Sheep, 20.20), Pull (Sweater, 15.15), Tricot (Knitting, 8.75), Doux (Soft, 5.72), Vierge (Virgin, 2.36), Pelote (Ball, 2.02), Coton (Cotton, 1.01), Naturel (Natural, 1.01), Douillet (Cosy, 0.67), Pure (Pure, 0.67), Veste (Jacket, 0.67), Agneau (Lamb, 0.34), Chinée (Dye, 0.34), Couverture (Blanket, 0.34), Echarpe (Scarf, 0.34).

L4: Train ( Train ), Voyage (Travel, 14.48), Wagon (Railcar, 5.39), Locomotive (Locomotive, 2.69), Gare (Station, 2.36), Express (Express, 2.02), Sifflet (Whistle, 1.68), Vapeur (Vapor, 1.68), Electrique (Electric, 1.01), Marchandises (Goods, 0.67), Retard (Delay, 0.67), Ticket (Ticket, 0.67), Auto-couchette (Car sleeper train, 0.34), Métro (Subway, 0.34), Omnibus (Local train, 0.34), Transsibérien (Tran-Siberian, 0.34).

L5: Voiture ( Car ), Vitesse (Speed, 6.06), Auto (Automobile, 4.04), Pollution (Pollution, 2.36), Accident (Accident, 1.68), Locomotion (Locomotion, 1.68), Rapide (Fast, 1.68), Essence (Gasoline, 1.35), Moteur (Engine, 1.35), Véhicule (Vehicle, 1.01), Panne (Breakdown of car, 0.67), Accélérateur (Accelerator, 0.34), Amende (Fine, 0.34), Conduire (Drive, 0.34), Décapotable (Convertible, 0.34), Guimbarde (Clunker, 0.34).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Evrard, C., Gilet, AL., Colombel, F. et al. Now you make false memories; now you do not: the order of presentation of words in DRM lists influences the production of the critical lure in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Research 82, 429–438 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0831-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0831-9

Navigation