Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Socially triggered negative affect impairs performance in simple cognitive tasks

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The aim of this research was to investigate the influence of a social-evaluative context on simple cognitive tasks. While another person present in the room evaluated photographs of beautiful women or landscapes by beauty/attractiveness, female participants had to perform a combination of digit-categorization and spatial-compatibility task. There, before every trial, one of the women or landscape pictures was presented. Results showed selective performance impairments: the numerical distance effects increased on trials that followed women pictures but only, if another person concurrently evaluated these women pictures. In a second experiment, using the affective priming paradigm, the authors show that female pictures have a more negative connotation when they are concurrently evaluated by another person (social-evaluative context) than when they are not evaluated (neutral context). Together, these results suggest that the social-evaluative context triggers mild negative affective reactions to women pictures which then impair performance in an unrelated task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Eliminating one shortcoming of our previous study, this time, we already presented both picture types in Block 1 of the cognitive task (landscape evaluation).

  2. This larger compatibility effect in the social-evaluation might be taken as evidence for a general distraction by the concurrent evaluation at the beginning of the experiment.

  3. Note however, that Aharon et al. (2001) found the effect for male participants only.

  4. The reason why we used a different outlier correction procedure in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1 was that the tasks used in both experiments are qualitatively different: in two-choice RT tasks, like the digit task in Experiment 1, it is common practice to use cutoff procedures based on the individual distributions (in our case a 3-SD cutoff). In contrast, due to the automaticity of the evaluation processes underlying the affective priming paradigm, it is common to use stricter cutoff values (e.g., Wentura, 1999; Strick et al. 2009). Therefore, the fact that the main results of Experiment 2 would not be significant anymore, if the 3 SD-criterion from Experiment 1 was used instead of the fixed < 2,000-ms cutoff, does not pose any problem to the validity of our results. Applying the 2,000-ms cutoff to Experiment 1 before the 3-SD cutoff did not have any qualitative effect on the data pattern.

References

  • Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: MRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32, 537–551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood–creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P., Fujii, M., & Kayra-Stuart, F. (1976). Semantic congruity effects in comparative judgments of magnitudes of digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 435–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and problems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influence in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cognitive Science, 4, 215–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 351–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626–641.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How “social” is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 84.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dreisbach, G., & Böttcher, S. (2011). How the social-evaluative context modulates processes of cognitive control. Psychological Research, 75, 143–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic. Why the adjustments are insufficient. Psychological Science, 17, 311–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: Conflict-adaption effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 712–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broadens, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K. (2003). When necessity is the mother of invention: Mood and problem solving. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 248–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 32–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, T. R., & Noice, H. (1988). Influence of positive affect upon creative thinking and problem solving in children. Psychological Reports, 63, 895–898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1106–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 20, 794–798.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huguet, P., Gavaing, M. P., Monteil, J. M., & Dumas, F. (1999). Social presence effects in the Stroop task: Further evidence for an attentional view of social facilitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1011–1025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. S., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. E., Mitchell, M. A., Bean, M. G., Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2010). Gender moderates the self-regulatory consequences of suppressing emotional reactions to sexism. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 13, 215–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kampe, K. K. W., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, U. (2001). Reward value of attractiveness and gaze. Nature, 413, 589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karremans, J. C., Verwijmeren, T., Pronk, T. M., & Reitsma, M. (2009). Interacting with women can impair men’s executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1041–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. C., Herfordt, J., & Voss, A. (2008). Social presence effects on the Stroop task: Boundary conditions and an alternative account. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 469–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M., & Karabenick, S. A. (1974). Physical attractiveness, body attitudes, and self-concept in late adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 3, 307–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. T. (1989). Processing deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 27–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. L. C., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 617–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequalities. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, D., Atzeni, T., & Butera, F. (2004). Coaction and upward social comparison reduce the illusory conjunction effect: Support for distraction-conflict theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 659–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T. (2007). Assimilation and contrast as comparison effects: A selective accessibility model. In D. A. Stapel & J. Suls (Eds.), Assimilation and contrast in social psychology (pp. 165–185). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T., Rüter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The man who wasn’t there. Subliminal social standards influence self-evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 689–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oriet, C., Tombu, M., & Jolicœur, P. (2005). Symbolic ND affects two processing loci in the number comparison task. Memory & Cognition, 33, 913–926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Padmala, S., Bauer, A., & Pessoa, L. (2011). Negative emotion impairs conflict-driven executive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 192. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00192.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, L. H., Bull, R., Adams, E., & Fraser, L. (2002). Positive mood and executive functions: Evidence from stroop and fluency tasks. Emotion, 2, 12–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prabhakaran, R., & Gray, J. R. (2012). The pervasive nature of unconscious social information processing in executive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(April), 1–12. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory, and application. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92–120). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 353–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states and cognitive task performance. Memory & Cognition, 19, 507–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, D., Booth, R., Brown, R., & Huguet, P. (2010). Exploring the temporal dynamics of social facilitation in the Stroop task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 52–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, M., Hajak, G., Döhnel, K., Meinhardt, J., & Müller, J. L. (2008). Emotion-dependent modulation of interference processes: An fMRI study. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 68, 193–203.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strick, M., Holland, R. W., & van Knippenberg, A. (2008). Seductive eyes: Attractiveness and direct gaze increase desire for associated objects. Cognition, 106, 1487–1496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strick, M., van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., & van Knippenberg, A. (2009). Humor in advertisements enhances product liking by mere association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 3–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolma, D. L., Impett, E. A., Tracy, A. J., & Michael, A. (2006). Looking good, sounding good: Femininity ideology and adolescent girls’ mental health. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 85–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tryon, W. W. (2001). Evaluating statistical difference, equivalence, and indeterminancy using inferential confidence intervals: An integrated alternative method of conducting null hypothesis statistical tests. Psychological Methods, 6, 371–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Võ, M. L., Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). The Berlin affective word list reloaded (BAWL-R). Behavior Research Methods, 41, 534–538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of affective information: Evidence for negative priming in the evaluation task. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 65–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wühr, P., & Huestegge, L. (2010). The impact of social presence on voluntary and involuntary control of spatial attention. Social Cognition, 28, 145–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Svenja Böttcher.

Additional information

We would like to thank Michaela Brandl, Julia Fritz and Constanze Schreiner for data collection.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Böttcher, S., Dreisbach, G. Socially triggered negative affect impairs performance in simple cognitive tasks. Psychological Research 78, 151–165 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0488-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0488-6

Keywords

Navigation