Skip to main content
Log in

Jump on the innovator’s train: cognitive principles for creating appreciation in innovative product designs

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Research in Engineering Design Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In everyday life, we find shared preferences for idiosyncratic product features paradigmatically displayed by bestselling gadgets like Apples iPhone’s touch screen, which after gaining acceptance and appreciation are susceptible to being copied by competitors. Psychological research on the phenomenon of shared preferences for innovative design features and the probable benefit of copying them is still lacking. We tested gains of acceptance for imitators through an adaptation paradigm where typicality and liking of potentially innovative features were analysed dynamically. We found significant changes in typicality and liking for imitators being highly similar to the original. These adaptation processes in combination with transfer effects create the specific opportunity for imitators to jump on the innovator’s train by providing similar innovative features and thereby participating in the initial innovator’s success. Importantly, they participate best not by solely copying a specific novel feature, but by additionally generally looking very similar to the innovator.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. None of the participants could assign the name, designer or brand name of the following models: Rocking Armchair “Rod” of Charles and Ray Eames, “Wassily No. B3” of Marcel Breuer, DSW” of Charles and Ray Eames, “Aluminium Chair EA 105” of Charles and Ray Eames, “LC4” of Le Corbusier,“Chair Cesca B64” of Marcel Breuer, “Lounge Chair and Ottoman” of Charles and Ray Eames,“LC2” of Le Corbusier,“Joe” of Gionatan De Pas, Donatod’ Urbino and Paolo Lomazzi, “Hill House Chair” of Charles Rennie Mackintosh, “Panton Chair” of Verner Panton, “La Chaise” of Charles and Ray Eames, “Marshmallow Sofa” of George Nelson, “Barcelona chair No. MR90” of Mies van der Rohe, “Armchair No. MR20” of Mies van der Rohe, “Wiggle Side Chair” of Frank Gehry, “Rood blauwestoel” of Gerrit Rietveld, “Stuhl No. 14” of Michael Thonet, “W. W. Stool” of Philippe Starck,and “Tulip Chair” of Eero Saarinen.

  2. Participants rated the adaptation stimuli on the following 24 attributes in the first adaptation phase: appealing (ansprechend), carefully thought out (durchdacht), classic (klassisch), compact (kompakt), conventional (konventionell), durable (beständig), elegant (elegant), extravagant (extravagant), formal (förmlich), functional (funktionell), futuristic (futuristisch), inviting (einladend), neat (ordentlich), of high quality (hochwertig), embellished/playful (verspielt), overwhelming (erdrückend), pleasant (angenehm), dull (eintönig), regular (regelmäßig), restrained (dezent), rounded (abgerundet), solid (gediegen), tasteful (geschmackvoll) and stuffy (bieder). The second adaptation phase comprised the following 12 attributes: bulky (sperrig), clear (klar), comfortable (komfortabel), conservative (konservativ), well considered (überlegt), practicable (praktisch), luxurious (luxuriös), minimalist (schlicht), modern (modern), robust (robust), stylish (stilvoll) and inventive (phantasievoll).

References

  • Barrett SE, O'Toole AJ (2009) Face adaptation to gender: does adaptation transfer across age categories? Vis Cogn 17:700–715

    Google Scholar 

  • Blijlevens J, Carbon CC, Mugge R, Schoormans JPL (2012) Aesthetic appraisal of product designs: independent effects of typicality and arousal. Br J Psychol 103:44–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein RF (1989) Exposure and affect: overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychol Bull 106:265–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein RF, Kale AR, Cornell KR (1990) Boredom as a limiting condition on the mere exposure effect. J Personal Soc Psychol 58:791–800

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G, DeBruine LM, Little AC, Welling LLM, Conway CA, Tiddeman BP, Jones BC (2006) Visual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness. Evol Human Behav 27:381–389

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC (2010) The cycle of preference: long-term dynamics of aesthetic appreciation. Acta Psychol 134:233–244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC (2011) Cognitive mechanisms for explaining dynamics of aesthetic appreciation. iPerception 2:708–719

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC, Ditye T (2011) Sustained effects of adaptation on the perception of familiar faces. J Exp Psychol Human Perform Percept 37:615–625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC, Ditye T (2012) Face adaptation effects show strong and long-lasting transfer from lab to more ecological contexts. Percept Sci 3:1–6

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC, Leder H (2005) The repeated evaluation technique (RET). A method to capture dynamic effects of innovativeness and attractiveness. Appl Cogn Psychol 19:587–601

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC, Schoormans JPL (2012) Rigidity rather than age as a limiting factor to appreciate innovative design. Swiss J Psychol 71:51–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carbon CC, Michael L, Leder H (2008) Design evaluation by combination of repeated evaluation technique and measurement of electrodermal activity. Res Eng Design 19:143–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen H, Russell R, Nakayama K, Livingstone M (2010) Crossing the ‘uncanny valley’: adaptation to cartoon faces can influence perception of human faces. Perception 39:378–386

    Google Scholar 

  • Faerber SJ, Carbon CC (2012) The power of liking: highly sensitive aesthetic processing for guiding us through the world. iPerception 3:553–561

    Google Scholar 

  • Faerber SJ, Leder H, Gerger G, Carbon CC (2010) Priming semantic concepts affects the dynamics of aesthetic appreciation. Acta Psychol 135:191–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerger G, Leder H, Faerber SJ, Carbon CC (2011) When the others matter: context-dependent effects on changes in appreciation of innovativeness. Swiss J Psychol 70:75–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hekkert P, Snelders D, van Wieringen PCW (2003) ‘Most advanced, yet acceptable’: typicality and novelty as joint predictors of aesthetic preference in industrial design. Br J Psychol 94:111–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard TJ, Dekoninck EA, Culley SJ (2010) The use of creative stimuli at early stages of industrial product innovation. Res Eng Design 21:263–274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchison KA (2003) Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychon Bull Rev 10:785–813

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakesch M, Zachhuber M, Leder H, Spingler M, Carbon CC (2011) Scenario-based touching: on the influence of top-down processes on tactile and visual appreciation. Res Eng Design 22:143–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreuzbauer R, Malter AJ (2005) Embodied cognition and new product design: changing product form to influence brand categorization. J Prod Innov Manage 22:165–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leder H, Carbon CC (2005) Dimensions in appreciation of car interior design. Appl Cogn Psychol 19:603–618

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loken B, Ross I, Hinkle RL (1986) Consumer confusion of origin and brand similarity perceptions. J Public Policy Mark 5:195–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Mengoni M, Germani M (2009) Reverse engineering and restyling of aesthetic products based on sketches interpretation. Res Eng Design 20:79–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulson T, Sproles G (2000) Styling strategy. Bus Horiz 43:45–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawsthorn A (2006) Apple's quest to put us at ease with technology: style and design, in The New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/2006/11/26/style/26iht-design27.3666131.html

  • Rhodes G, Jeffery L, Watson TL, Clifford CWG, Nakayama K (2003) Fitting the mind to the world: face adaptation and attractiveness after effects. Psychol Sci 14:558–566

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes G, Louw K, Evangelista E (2009) Perceptual adaptation to facial asymmetries. Psychon Bull Rev 16:503–508

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers EM (2003) Diffusion of innovations. Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • van Horen F, Pieters R (2012) When high-similarity copycats lose and moderate-similarity copycats gain: the impact of comparative evaluation. J Mark Res 49:83–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warlop L, Alba JW (2004) Sincere flattery: trade-dress imitation and consumer choice. J Consumer Psychol 14:21–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc RB (1968) Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc Psychol 9:1–27

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Andrea Lyman and Stefanie Wolz for proofreading the text and Sabine Albrecht, JörnFreese, Claudia Gorr, Sabine Kügel and David Winkelmann for conducting parts of the experimental work. Last but not the least, we wish to thank Yoram Reich and two anonymous reviewers for providing valuable comments and constructive criticism for an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stella J. Faerber.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Faerber, S.J., Carbon, CC. Jump on the innovator’s train: cognitive principles for creating appreciation in innovative product designs. Res Eng Design 24, 313–319 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00163-012-0148-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00163-012-0148-7

Keywords

Navigation