Skip to main content
Log in

Acquired dyslexia in a turkish-english speaker

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Turkish script is characterised by completely transparent bidirectional mappings between orthography and phonology. To date, there has been no reported evidence of acquired dyslexia in Turkish speakers leading to the naïve view that reading and writing problems in Turkish are probably rare. We examined the extent to which phonological impairment and orthographic transparency influence reading disorders in a native Turkish speaker. BRB is a bilingual Turkish-English speaker with deep dysphasia accompanied by acquired dyslexia in both languages. The main findings are an effect of imageability on reading in Turkish coincident with surface dyslexia in English and preserved nonword reading. BRB’s acquired dyslexia suggests that damage to phonological representations might have a consequence for learning to read in Turkish. We argue that BRB’s acquired dyslexia has a common locus in chronic underactivation of phonological representations in Turkish and English. Despite a common locus, reading problems manifest themselves differently according to properties of the script and the type of task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Angelelli, P., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2004). Characteristics of writing disorders in Italian dyslexic children. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17, 18–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (1991). Visual word recognition: Evidence for strategic control of lexical and nonlexical routines in oral reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 644–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, M. F., & Derouesne, J. (1979). Phonological alexia: Three dissociations. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 42, 1115–1124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beland, R., & Mimouni, Z. (2001). Deep dyslexia in the two languages of an Arabic/French bilingual patient. Cognition, 82, 77–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D. (1999). Basic processes in reading: Multiple routines in localist and connectionist models. In P. A. McMullen & R. M. Klein (Eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia (pp. 413–458). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breedin, S. D., Saffran, E. M., & Coslett, H. B. (1994). Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 617–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1980a). Deep dyslexia: A review of the syndrome. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 22–47). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1980b). Deep dyslexia: A right hemisphere hypothesis. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 326–380). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart M. (1984). Writing systems and reading disorders. In L. Henderson (Ed.), Orthographies and reading: Perspectives from cognitive psychology, neuropsychology, and linguistics (pp. 67–80). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2000). Deep dyslexia is right-hemisphere reading. Brain and Language, 71, 299–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Masterson, J., Byng, S., Prior, M., & Riddoch, M. J. (1983). Surface dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 469–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cortese, M., & Simpson, G. (2000). Regularity effects in word naming: What are they? Memory and Cognition, 28, 1269–1276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., & Labos, E. (2001). The autonomy of the orthographic pathway in a shallow language: Data from an aphasic patient. Aphasiology, 15(4), 333–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diesfeldt, H. F. A. (1992). Impaired and preserved semantic memory functions in dementia. In L. Beckman (Ed.), Memory functioning in dementia. Advances in psychology, 89 (pp. 227–263). Oxford, England: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijkstra, A., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (1998). The BIA-model and bilingual word recognition. In J. Grainger & A. M. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 189–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoğlu, A. Y. (2002). Cross-linguistic transfer in literacy development and implications for language learners. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 189–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, J., & Pring, L. (2004). The processing of inflectional morphology: A comparison of children with and without dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 567–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eng, N., & Obler, L. K. (2002). Acquired dyslexia in a biscript reader following traumatic brain injury: A second case. Topics in Language Disorders, 22(5), 5–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, S., Turner, J., Lambon-Ralph, M. A., & Morris, J. (1996). A distinctive case of word meaning deafness? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 1139–1162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Funnell, E. (1983). Phonological processes in reading: New evidence from acquired dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 159–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2003). Phonology, learning to read and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic analysis. In V. Csepe (Ed.), Dyslexia: Different brain, different behaviour (pp. 1–40). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, K. S., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. E. (1994). The relationship between comprehension and oral reading in progressive fluent aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 32, 299–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. (1982). Orthography and word recognition in reading. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillis, A., & Caramazza, A. (1991). Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category specific semantic deficit. Brain and Language, 40, 106–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillis, A. E., & Caramazza, A. (1995). Converging evidence for the interaction of semantic and sublexical phonological information in accessing lexical representation for spoken output. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 187–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (1996). The effects of polysemy in lexical decision and naming: An alternative to lexical access accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1331–1356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iribarren, I. C., Jarema, G., & Lecours, A. R. (1999). Lexical reading in Spanish: Two cases of phonological dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 2–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambon-Ralph, M., Ellis, A. W., & Sage, K. (1998). Word meaning blindness revisited. Cognitive-Neuropsychology, 15, 389–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law, S. P., & Or, B. (2001). A case study of acquired dyslexia and dysgraphia in Cantonese: Evidence for nonsemantic pathways for reading and writing Chinese. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 729–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K., & Parkinson, M. E. (1995). Processing of English morphological structure by poor readers. In C. K. Leong & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Developmental and acquired dyslexia: Neuropsychological and neurolinguistic perspectives (pp. 237–261). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Majerus, S., Lekeu, F., Van der Linden, M., & Salmon, E. (2001). Deep dysphasia: Further evidence on the relationship between phonological short term memory and language processing impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 385–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Dell, G. S (1996). Recovery in deep dysphasia: Evidence for a relation between auditory verbal STM capacity and lexical errors in repetition. Brain and Language, 52, 83–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masterson, J., Coltheart, M., & Meara, P. (1985). Surface dyslexia in a language without irregularly spelled words. In K. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart, (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 215–223). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCann, R. S., & Besner, D. (1987). Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation and the locus of word frequency effects in word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 14–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miceli, G., & Caramazza, A. (1993). The assignment of word stress in oral reading: Evidence from a case of acquired dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 273–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monaghan, J., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). What exactly interacts with spelling-sound consistency in word naming? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28, 183–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newton, P. K., & Barry, C. (1997). Concreteness effects in word production but not word comprehension in deep dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 481–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öney, B., & Durgunoğlu, A. Y. (1997). Beginning to read Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1992). Deterioration of word meaning: Implications for reading. Neuropsychologia. 30, 1025–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. E., Graham, N., & Hodges, J. R. (1994). Reading in dementia of the Alzheimer type: A preserved ability? Neuropsychology, 8, 395–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K., Suzuki, T., Wydell, T., & Sasanuma, S. (1995). Progressive aphasia and surface alexia in Japanese. Neurocase, 1, 155–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., Demonet, J. F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cossu, G., Habib, M., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2001). Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291(5511), 2165–2167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2000). Cross-language computational investigation of the length effect in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 990–1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles of quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I. (2003). Lexicality effects in single-word naming in alphabetic Turkish orthography. In M. Joshi, C. K. Leong, & B. L. J. Kaczmarek (Eds.), Literacy acquisition: Role of phonology, morphology and orthography (pp. 83–93). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I., & Baluch, B. (2001). Semantic effects as a function of reading skill in word naming of a transparent orthography. Reading & Writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 599–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I., Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (2004). On the control of visual word recognition: Changing routes versus changing deadlines. Memory and Cognition, 32, 489–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raman, I., & Weekes, B. (2003). Deep dysphasia in Turkish. Brain and Language. 87, 38–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3(2), 212–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F., Marin, O. S., & Saffran, E. M. (1979). Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language. 7, 277–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Reading without semantics. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(A), 111–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 431(7004), 71–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smythe, I., Everatt, J., & Salter, R. (Eds.). (2004). The International book of dyslexia: A cross language comparison and practice guide. Chichester, UK: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 1140–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valdois, S., Carbonnel, S., David, D., Rousset, S., & Pellat, J. (1995). Confrontation of PDP models and dual route models through the analysis of a case of deep dysphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 681–724.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1979). Semantic access dyslexia. Brain, 102, 43–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, S., Jokel, R., & Behrmann, M. (1997). Surface dyslexia in nonfluent progressive aphasia. Brain and Language, 56, 211–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (2005). Dyslexia and dysgraphia among Spanish speakers: A cognitive neuropsychological approach. In J. G. Centeno, L. K. Obler, & R. Anderson (Eds.), Studying communication disorders in Spanish speakers: Theoretical, research & clinical aspects. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S., & Chen, H. Q. (1999). Surface dyslexia in Chinese. Neurocase, 5, 161–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Surface dyslexia and surface dysgraphia: Treatment studies and their theoretical implications. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 277–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B., Coltheart, M., & Gordon, E. (1997). Deep dyslexia and right hemisphere reading—a regional cerebral blood flow study. Aphasiology, 11, 1139–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S., & Robinson, G. (1997). Semantic anomia without surface dyslexia. Aphasiology, 11, 813–825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wijk, A. (1966). Rules of pronunciation for the English language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wydell, T. N., & Butterworth, B. (1999). A case study of an English-Japanese bilingual with monolingual dyslexia. Cognition, 70, 273–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, W. G., & Weekes, B. S. (2003). Dyslexia in Chinese: Clues from cognitive neuropsychology. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 255–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 413–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., & Braun, M. (2001). Identical words are read differently in different languages. Psychological Science, 12, 379–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brendan S. Weekes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Raman, I., Weekes, B.S. Acquired dyslexia in a turkish-english speaker. Ann. of Dyslexia 55, 79–104 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0005-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0005-8

Key Words

Navigation