Introduction

Obesity is a major risk factor for the development of type 2 diabetes in adults and children. With the growing prevalence of childhood obesity, the incidence of type 2 diabetes in children and adolescents is increasing at an alarming rate [1]. Type 2 diabetes may become the most common form of paediatric diabetes within the next decade. Older adults with type 2 diabetes are at increased risk for developing micro- and macrovascular complications (e.g. retinopathy, neuropathy, nephropathy and cardiovascular disease) [2]. They also show significant cognitive slowing as well as learning and memory deficits [3, 4]. Structural brain abnormalities are also commonly associated with type 2 diabetes among older adults [57] but for the most part, those abnormalities tend to reflect non-specific pathology such as increased atrophy, overt white matter pathology or stroke [3, 8], and their magnitude tends to be modest. However, more recent studies have demonstrated significantly reduced hippocampal volumes among middle-aged and elderly individuals with type 2 diabetes [9, 10].

There have been only a few brain studies in children and young adults with type 1 diabetes, and the findings are so far inconsistent, with one study showing reduced brain volume relative to matched non-diabetic controls [11] but another showing no differences [12]. Young people with type 2 diabetes, despite shorter disease duration and better glucose control, have higher rates of complications (microalbuminuria and hypertension) than their peers with type 1 diabetes [13]. Therefore, we predict that their brains may also be more affected. However, no studies have evaluated cognition or the brain in children or adolescents with type 2 diabetes. Although adolescents with type 2 diabetes do not yet have clinically significant vascular disease, there is evidence that they may have increased carotid artery intima-media thickness and stiffness [14], which has also been reported among children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes [15].

Microstructural integrity of cerebral white and grey matter (WM and GM) has been associated with intact cognitive performance in both adults and children [16, 17]. Although not anticipating finding gross WM disease among obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes, we were interested in exploring whether there are WM microstructural alterations that could adversely affect cognitive functioning in these still-developing brains. Consequently, we used sensitive and unbiased magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques, such as diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and statistical parametric mapping (SPM)-based segmentation and volumetric analysis, to determine the presence of subtle cerebral alterations among our participants.

The goal of this study was to ascertain, for the first time, whether relative to demographically similar non-diabetic obese adolescents with no evidence of marked insulin resistance, obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes have diminished cognitive performance and subtle abnormalities in brain integrity. Given that obesity is one of the major risk factors for sleep apnoea in children [18] and that obstructive sleep apnoea is known to affect both cognition [19] and the brain [20], the groups were also matched on self-ratings of sleep apnoea. We hypothesised that adolescents with type 2 diabetes would show reductions on measures of overall intellectual function, declarative memory, executive function and psychomotor efficiency. In addition, we predicted that they would have diffuse alterations in cerebral WM and GM integrity, particularly in the frontal and medial temporal regions.

Methods

Participants

We studied 18 obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes consecutively referred by local area paediatric endocrinologists. Eighteen obese adolescents without type 2 diabetes or marked insulin resistance comparable in age, sex, school grade, ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES) (AB Hollingshead, Department of Sociology, Yale University, unpublished data), BMI and waist circumference were recruited as controls among relatives of other study participants or via internet advertisements. This study was approved by the local Internal Review Board. Informed consent was obtained from parents and assent obtained from each child. The data acquisition for this study was completed in 2009.

All participants underwent medical, endocrine, psychiatric, neuropsychological and brain MRI assessments during a comprehensive 6–7 h evaluation completed over two visits. We excluded individuals with significant medical conditions (other than type 2 diabetes, lipid abnormalities, polycystic ovary disease or hypertension). As we do not anticipate that adolescents with type 2 diabetes or obese controls will have sufficient ischaemic microvessel disease to result in extensive WM hyperintensities on MRI, any occurrence of significant WM hyperintensities in this age group is likely to be indicative of a demyelinating condition such as multiple sclerosis, and this clearly warrants exclusion. We therefore excluded individuals with extensive WM pathology. Also excluded were adolescents with: sexual development Tanner stage less than 4; a psychiatric diagnosis such as depression; current use of psychoactive medications; mental retardation; or significant learning disability.

Standard diagnostic criteria for type 2 diabetes were used and all were beta cell antibody negative. Fasting glucose and insulin values were used to compute HOMA-insulin resistance (HOMA-IR) [21], which has been demonstrated to be a reasonable screening measure of insulin resistance relative to the hyperinsulinaemic–euglycaemic clamp assessment [22]. Control participants had a HOMA-IR ≤ 2.50, which indicated that they were not likely to have marked insulin resistance. Participants were classified as hypertensive if they received anti-hypertensive treatment or had a sitting blood pressure above the National Cholesterol Education Program cut-off (a systolic BP ≥ 130 mmHg or a diastolic BP ≥ 85 mmHg) [23]. The sleep-related behaviour questionnaire consists of 20 items to be answered ‘yes’, ‘no’, or ‘don’t know’ [24]. The self-rating of sleep apnoea depends on the proportion of questions answered yes.

Neuropsychological evaluations

The assessment was conducted in a standardised manner in the postprandial state, split over two sessions each lasting about 1.5 h. We used commonly used normed tests that have been described in detail by Lezak et al. [25]. Overall intellectual functioning was estimated from the Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subtests from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). Academic achievement was estimated from the Reading, Spelling and Arithmetic subtests from the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT). Verbal, visual and working memory skills were assessed with the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning (WRAML). Attention was measured with the accuracy score from the Digit Vigilance Test and the Attention/Concentration Index Score from the WRAML. Executive function was evaluated with the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), the Tower of London Test, and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT). Psychomotor efficiency was assessed with the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST). Raw scores are reported, with the exception of the WASI, WRAT and WRAML, which provide aged-corrected standard scores.

MR image acquisition and image analysis

Structural images

All participants were studied on the same 1.5 T Siemens Avanto MRI System (Berlin, Germany). Spin–lattice relaxation time (T1)-weighted magnetisation-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE; repetition time [TR] 1,300 ms; echo time [TE] 4.38 ms; inversion time [TI] 800 ms; field of view [FOV] 250 × 250; slice thickness 1.2 mm; number of averages [NEX] 1; flip angle 15°; matrix size 256 × 256; 192 coronal slices) and spin–spin relaxation time (T2)-weighted (TR 9,000 ms; TE 94 ms; TI 2,000 ms; FOV 210 × 210; slice thickness 3 mm; matrix size 256 × 256; 50 axial slices) sequences were used as anatomical guides for the DTI analysis. To rule out primary neurological disease and to quantify WM disease, the fast fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR; TR 9,000 ms; TE 97 ms; FOV 210 × 210; one average and two concatenations; flip angle 145°; slice thickness 3 mm; matrix size 256 × 256; 50 axial slices) image was used with the MPRAGE. The modified Fazekas scale [26] was used to rate WM hyperintensities, with scores >1 leading to study exclusion. To measure brain microstructural integrity a DTI echo planar sequence (TR 6,100 ms; TE 75 ms; delay in TR = 0 ms; b values 0, 1,000 s/mm2; six directions; FOV 210 × 210; four averages and one concatenation; slice thickness 3 mm; matrix size 128 × 128; 50 axial slices) was acquired. To optimise image co-registration, the DTI, T2-weighted and FLAIR images were all obtained in the same axial plane, using the same slice thickness and number.

Diffusion tensor imaging assessment of brain tissue

DTI uses the differential water diffusion properties of brain tissues to measure cerebral microstructural integrity and organisation. The more constraint to the movement of water, the more anisotropic the diffusion and the more intact the microstructural integrity of the tissue is likely to be. The most commonly used DTI index for assessing WM integrity is fractional anisotropy (FA), ranging in value from 0 (fully isotropic diffusion) to 1 (fully anisotropic diffusion) [27]. Another useful measure is the apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC), which represents the mean magnitude of water diffusivity, thus providing an assessment of tissue density. ADC elevations may represent increased extracellular space, a possible indication of neuronal damage [28], which makes it useful in GM regions.

WM/GM volumetric analysis

Spatial normalisation and segmentation of MPRAGE images used automated procedures as described in Good et al. [29] in the statistical parametric mapping software version 2 (SPM2). MPRAGE images were first corrected for signal non-uniformities and spatially normalised to the standard T1 Montreal Neurological Institute template. Using the tissue classification algorithm in SPM2, we segmented the normalised MPRAGE images into their GM, WM and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) partitions (see Fig. 1), which are maps representing the probability for each voxel being classified as GM, WM and CSF, respectively. These segmented partitions were subsequently normalised to their respective standard templates.

Fig. 1
figure 1

MPRAGE image segmentation. Individual MPRAGE images are registered to standard space before segmentation is accomplished. a Normalised MPRAGE image. b Segmented image: grey matter (red), white matter (green) and CSF (white) partitions are overlaid on top of the normalised MPRAGE image

In addition to performing whole-brain assessment, and given that during adolescence frontal lobe myelination is still ongoing [30], we also specifically assessed frontal lobes by using our published reliable frontal lobe parcellation method [31]. We quantified the proportions of WM, GM and CSF volumes in the whole brain and frontal regions at the case level by first mapping the region to each segmented partition and then averaging the values across participants for each of the two groups.

DTI image processing

To create the group maps for voxelwise group comparisons a three-step approach was used to correct the images for echo planar acquisition distortions after optimising registration between the T2 and MPRAGE images [32]. The FA and ADC values were computed and maps were generated from the native DTI sequence. Finally, to reduce interpolation errors, all transformations were combined into a single step, which was applied to the FA and ADC maps generated from the native DTI data. Refer to Yau et al. for a detailed description of these methods [33].

Statistical analysis

We conducted two-tailed independent sample t tests examining group differences in demographics, endocrine data, cognitive data and brain volumes. Variables that were not normally distributed were also tested using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. The χ 2 test examined group difference in SES. For the imaging data two-tailed voxelwise analysis of covariance (VANCOVA) analyses were performed to examine group differences in whole-brain WM FA and GM ADC, with age as a covariate. WM and GM masks, created from the average MPRAGE image of all participants, were used to confine FA and ADC analyses within those regions. To minimise the chance of type I errors, we restricted the accepted cluster size to those having at least 100 contiguous voxels (each voxel is 1 mm3, thus, the minimum cluster size is 0.1 ml in volume) and by then choosing a false discovery rate (FDR) less than 0.01. We chose a p value threshold of 0.005 to ensure that the FDR would be kept below 0.01 [34].

Results

Demographic and endocrine data

There were no group differences in age, sex, school grade, SES, BMI, waist circumference, waist–height ratio or ratings of sleep apnoea. Groups were also comparable in ethnicity. As expected, relative to obese controls, adolescents with type 2 diabetes had significantly higher fasting glucose and insulin levels, HOMA-IR, HbA1c and triacylglycerols, and significantly lower HDL cholesterol levels (p < 0.05; Table 1). The groups did not differ in total cholesterol, LDL cholesterol, or high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (CRP). Five adolescents with type 2 diabetes and four obese controls had elevated blood pressure, but only one adolescent with diabetes was receiving pharmacological treatment for it. Diabetic participants had been diagnosed with type 2 diabetes for 2.61 ± 1.90 years (range 5 months–7.50 years). The groups did not differ on self-ratings of obstructive sleep apnoea.

Table 1 Demographic and endocrine data

Neuropsychological results

Adolescents with type 2 diabetes consistently scored lower than obese controls on all cognitive tests administered (Table 2). They had significantly lower overall intellectual functioning, lower verbal memory scores, lower psychomotor efficiency (all with p < 0.05), and tended to have more perseverative errors on the WCST (p = 0.06). Note that all of these measures showed medium-large to large effect sizes. Despite no group differences in school grade or SES, diabetic participants also performed worse on the WRAT achievement tests, reading (p = 0.09) and spelling (p = 0.07), both trending toward significance and with medium effect sizes. The three cognitive tests (i.e. the WASI, the Digit Vigilance Test and the WCST) that were not normally distributed in one or both of the groups were also analysed using non-parametric statistics (Kolmogorov–Smirnov) and the results were identical; therefore, for ease of presentation in Table 2 we show only the results from the t tests. Additionally, we confirmed that ratings of sleep apnoea were not associated with any of the cognitive measures that differentiated the groups.

Table 2 Neuropsychological results

MRI results

Two control participants had missing MRI scans. Of the 34 participants with MRI scans, only one adolescent with type 2 diabetes had periventricular punctate hyperintensities (rated 1 on the modified Fazekas scale). This very subtle level of abnormality is not considered clinically significant and was not mentioned in this individual’s neuroradiological reports.

Volumetric analysis of the segmented partitions revealed that there were no GM volume reductions, but obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes had significantly smaller WM volumes and enlarged CSF space for the whole brain and frontal lobe (p ≤ 0.005; all with large effect sizes). These differences remained significant after adjusting for age (Table 3).

Table 3 Brain volumetric data

DTI VANCOVA revealed both WM and GM microstructural abnormalities among diabetic participants, independent of age (Table 4). When we contrasted the two groups, we found that among obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes there was a total of five clusters of WM FA reductions (p < 0.005) with a total volume of 725 voxels or 0.725 ml. The type 2 diabetes group only had one small cluster demonstrating significant WM FA elevation. In addition, there were 27 clusters showing GM ADC elevations (p < 0.005) among adolescents with type 2 diabetes, 20 of which were in the cerebrum. The cerebral clusters had a total volume of 4,012 voxels or 4.01 ml. In exploratory analyses with a less conservative p value threshold of 0.01, we identified a total of 67 FA or ADC significant clusters, only five of which showed higher FA or lower ADC values in type 2 diabetes.

Table 4 Clusters of FA reductions or ADC elevations in type 2 diabetes (p < 0.005)

Given the extensive number of clusters of abnormality, we chose to only itemise in Table 4 the largest clusters using the more conservative p value threshold of less than 0.005. These same sets of selected clusters are displayed on an average brain structural image in Figs 2a (FA) and 2b (ADC).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Sampling of significant FA and ADC clusters. The three rows of images, each showing an orthogonal orientation (axial, coronal, and sagittal), display the clusters with the axes going through the centroid (coordinates in Table 4). (a) WM FA reductions (blue) and (b) GM ADC elevations (yellow/orange) in the type 2 diabetes group

Discussion

This is the first report of brain and cognitive abnormalities among obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes. We demonstrate that, in the absence of clinically significant vascular disease, there may be clear brain complications among adolescents with type 2 diabetes. These present predominantly as whole-brain and frontal WM volume reductions and diffuse WM and GM microstructural abnormalities. In addition, we find that relative to matched obese controls, general intellectual ability, verbal memory, executive function, psychomotor efficiency and measures of academic achievement—particularly reading and spelling skills—are lower among adolescents with type 2 diabetes. The intellectual capacity and verbal memory impairments are consistent with those reported among middle-aged adults with well-controlled type 2 diabetes [9], but unlike adults with type 2 diabetes, the reductions in cognitive performance were not restricted to those two domains.

Our DTI analyses demonstrated that, compared with relatively insulin-sensitive obese controls, obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes showed FA reductions in WM tracts in the temporal stem, cingulate, frontal lobe, occipital lobe and cerebral peduncles. During adolescence, the brain continues to undergo the morphological and functional changes associated with continued WM maturation [35], and because of this the observed WM volume and FA reductions may represent delayed WM maturation or axonal damage. In youths with type 1 diabetes, hyperglycaemia has been shown to be associated with smaller grey and white matter volumes despite no volume reduction relative to age-matched controls [12]. Hyperglycaemia-associated oxidative stress may also induce selective oligodendrocyte death, resulting in demyelination [36]. Although hypertension-related ischaemic damage remains a possible mechanism for WM damage [37], our voxelwise DTI results remained largely unchanged even after controlling for hypertension. In the absence of group differences in whole-brain or frontal GM volumes, our findings of diffuse ADC elevations in GM among diabetic individuals suggest reductions in GM density. The exact nature or degree of GM alterations remains to be better characterised with more direct assessments using techniques such as spectroscopy or assessment of tissue properties such as T1 relaxation.

We did not assess whether our participants had significant occlusive vascular disease as there is no clear rationale for anticipating clinically significant vascular abnormalities in adolescents. We propose that the negative impact of type 2 diabetes on the adolescent brain may result from a combination of functional vascular changes and glucose and lipid metabolism abnormalities in the absence of overt vascular disease. Type 2 diabetes is known to lead to an altered vascular physiology, such as reductions in endothelial dependent vasodilatation and deficits in cerebral vascular reactivity to CO2 [38]. During brain activation, such as occurs when performing a cognitive task, there is an increase in synaptic activity in the brain region involved. In the normal brain this results in regional vasodilation and thus an increase in glucose availability to that region to support the increased cognitive demand [39]. Therefore, vascular reactivity, which is integral to well-regulated cerebral blood flow, is key for maintaining an optimal neuronal environment during brain activation [40]. Although the underlying mechanisms remain elusive, we propose that impairments in vascular reactivity, which have been shown to be present among individuals with insulin resistance or type 2 diabetes [41, 42], may contribute to the reductions in cognitive performance present among obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes. Other diabetes-associated complications, such as subclinical large vessel and microvascular abnormalities, are among possible contributing factors [14, 43].

Although we did not uncover CRP differences between groups—most likely because our control group was also obese—we did not evaluate inflammation in a comprehensive manner, and inflammation remains a potentially important mechanism mediating some of the adverse effects of type 2 diabetes on brain integrity [44]. Future studies should also assess body composition in more detail, as more subtle differences in adiposity may also contribute to the effects observed.

The effect sizes for the cognitive differences were larger than those reported in studies of adolescents with type 1 diabetes and adults with type 2 diabetes. Though alarming, our results are consistent with the higher complication rate in peripheral tissues in those with type 2 diabetes relative to their adolescent peers with type 1 diabetes, despite shorter disease duration [13]. Also supporting these larger effect sizes in the degree of abnormality, adolescents with type 2 diabetes have a markedly reduced life expectancy accompanied by a dramatic increase in medical complications [45]. Our diabetic participants had a mean HbA1c of 8.3%, indicating intermediate glycaemic control. Given adult data demonstrating cognitive impairments during prediabetic stages of insulin resistance [46], it is possible that subtle alterations begin during the preclinical stages, thus increasing the time of adverse exposure for these young brains. The lack of associations between ratings of obstructive sleep apnoea and cognitive performance may be explained by the fact that obesity is closely associated with sleep apnoea in children [18] and our groups were matched on both BMI and waist circumference. Future studies should measure sleep apnoea more comprehensively.

This study has two major strengths: our groups comprised adolescents with similar demographic and socioeconomic backgrounds as well as comparable BMI and waist circumference. A second strength is that our automated brain analysis methods provide unbiased, sensitive and comprehensive assessments of both gross and microstructural brain structure. This study is, however, limited by its relatively small sample size and the fact that we carried out 13 univariate comparisons for the cognitive data and six comparisons for the brain volumes, and did not correct for multiple comparisons. However, all cognitive variables were descriptively lower in the diabetic group with 6/13 showing at least a statistical trend. In addition, 4/6 brain-volume variables differed significantly in the predicted direction. If we use the very conservative Bonferroni correction, the overall intellectual capacity difference and all the brain volume differences remain significant. Consequently, we consider it justified that, in this first report of brain and cognitive complications in adolescents with type 2 diabetes, we have not controlled for multiple comparisons.

This study demonstrates both brain and cognitive abnormalities among obese adolescents with type 2 diabetes. To better understand the underlying pathological mechanisms, future work should include assessments of vascular function, dynamic insulin function, lipid metabolism and inflammatory markers, as well as their interactions. Furthermore, future work should be longitudinal, thus tracking pathological changes from the preclinical stages of insulin resistance to type 2 diabetes. Given that some of our obese control participants are likely to have subtle levels of insulin resistance, it is possible that the group differences we uncovered would be even larger relative to lean adolescents. It will be essential to ascertain whether these brain abnormalities are reversible with improvements in diabetic control.