Skip to main content
Log in

Non-word Repetition Impairment in Autism and Specific Language Impairment: Evidence for Distinct Underlying Cognitive Causes

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Language-impaired individuals with autism perform poorly on tests such as non-word repetition that are sensitive clinical markers of specific language impairment (SLI). This has fuelled the theory that language impairment in autism represents a co-morbid SLI. However, the underlying cause of these deficits may be different in each disorder. In a novel task, we manipulated non-word stimuli in three ways known to influence the repetition accuracy of children with SLI. Participants with SLI were affected differently by these manipulations to children with autism. Children with autism performed similarly to language-matched typical children in terms of levels and patterns of performance, and types of error made, suggesting that the underlying cognitive cause of non-word repetition deficits is different in each disorder.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Three participants with ASD scored under the ASD cut-off of 15 on the SCQ, with scores of 12, 13, and 4, respectively. Two of these participants (who scored 12 and 13) were taking part in another of our studies and, as a result, had data from another measure of ASD severity; the Developmental, Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview (3di; Skuse et al. 2004), a more detailed parent interview schedule that is based on the Autism Diagnostic Interview (Le Couteur et al. 2003). Each of these participants scored well above the ASD cut-off on the 3di. The final participant (who scored 4 on the SCQ) did not have any other parent report data available. However, this participant had been diagnosed by a UK-leading clinician in London. This, combined with our clinical impression of the child, make us entirely confident of his diagnosis, regardless of his score on the SCQ.

  2. It may strike the reader as out of keeping with a diagnosis of specific language impairment that four participants in the SLI group had a NVIQ score outside of the normal range. However, a notable proportion of individuals who manifest SLI early in life (during the period in which most receive a diagnosis) show below average NVIQ later in life (e.g., Conti-Ramsden et al. 2001; Mawhood et al. 2000), with NVIQ dropping as much as 20 points across time (Botting 2005). Therefore, to obtain a representative sample of children who receive a diagnosis of SLI, we did not set as an inclusion criterion that NVIQ be in the normal range. We did, however, decide that in cases where NVIQ was below 80, non-verbal abilities must be substantially superior to verbal abilities, reflecting the fact that language problems are identified as the most clinically significant feature among all participants with SLI.

  3. In fact, it is important to note that ADOS-G and ADI-R appear to have a surprisingly low specificity, which raises questions about their suitability for use in research studies. In the largest study of its kind, Risi, Lord, Gotham, Corsello, Chrysler et al. (2006) found that, if used in isolation, the specificity of each measure was less than 50 %, with the identification of around 29 % of non-spectrum children as having autism. If used together, specificity is improved, but in over 15 % of cases the instruments disagree on spectrum vs. non-spectrum diagnoses. These measures (particularly the ADI) have come under recent scrutiny, with some offering what we view as persuasive arguments that measures such as the SCQ and 3di are preferable to ADOS and ADI for research purposes (Bishop 2011, May 30).

Abbreviations

ASD-LI:

Language-impaired individuals with autism spectrum disorder

VMA-TD:

Verbal mental age-matched typically developing individuals

CA-TD:

Chronological age-matched typically developing individuals

SLI:

Specific language impairment

CELF:

Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals

CLS:

Core language score

CNTNAP2:

Contactin associated protein-like 2

ATP2C2:

ATPase, Ca++ transporting, type 2C, member 2

CMIP:

c-Maf inducing protein

References

  • Alarcón, M., Abrahams, B. S., Stone, J. L., Duvall, J. A., Perederiy, J. V., Bomar, J. M., et al. (2008). Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 150–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edition, text revised) (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 919–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arking, D. E., Cutler, D. J., Brune, C. W., Teslovich, T. M., West, K., Ikeda, M., et al. (2008). A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 160–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: The Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ballarati, L., Recalcati, M. P., Bedeschi, M. F., Lalatta, F., Valtorta, C., Bellini, M., et al. (2009). Cytogenetic, FISH and array-CGH characterization of a complex chromosomal rearrangement carried by a mentally and language impaired patient. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 52, 218–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barry, J. G., Yasin, I., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Heritable risk factors associated with language impairments. Genes Brain and Behavior, 6, 66–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 1: Children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1977). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 3: Discriminant function analysis. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7(4), 383–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: Phenomimicry or shared etiology? Behavior Genetics, 40, 618–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. (2011, May 30). Are our ‘gold standard’ autism diagnostic instruments fit for purpose? [Web log message] Retrieved from http://deevybee.blogspot.com/2011/05/are-our-gold-standard-autism-diagnostic.html.

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Maybery, M., Wong, D., Maley, A., Hill, W., & Hallmayer, J. (2004). Are phonological processing deficits part of the broad autism phenotype? American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 128B, 54–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N. (2005). Non-verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 317–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, L. M., Kennedy, J. K. J., & Plomin, R. (2006). Generalist genes and cognitive neuroscience. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(14), 5–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coady, J., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2001). Follow-up of children attending infant language units: Outcomes at 11 years of age. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 207–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 251–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elia, J., Gai, X., Xie, H. M., Perin, J. C., Geiger, E., Glessner, J. T., et al. (2010). Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 637–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallon, N., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Nonword repetition: An investigation of phonological complexity in children with Grammatical-SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 435–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a casual connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Second Edition. London: Pearson Psychological Corporation.

  • Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. (2000). Specific language impairment across languages. In D. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention, and outcome (pp. 115–129). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

  • Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leventhal, B., DiLavore, B., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, C. R., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2003). The nature of phonological representations in children with Grammatical-Specific Language Impairment (G-SLI). In D. Hall, T. Markopoulos, A. Salamoura & S. Skoufaki (Eds.), Proceedings of the University of Cambridge first postgraduate conference in language research (Vol. 1, pp. 511–517).

  • Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). Effects of word position and stress on onset cluster production: Evidence from typical development, specific language impairment, and dyslexia. Language, 85, 39–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—A comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 547–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., & Frith, U. (1995). Causal modeling: A structural approach to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, volume 1, theory and methods (pp. 357–390). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A. J., et al. (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newbury, D. F., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Paracchini, S., Buckingham, L., Clark, A., et al. (2009). CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 264–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 23–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scerri, T. S., Morris, A. P., Buckingham, L. L., Newbury, D. F., Miller, L. L., Monaco, A. P., et al. (2011). DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 237–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-fourth edition (UK). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skuse, D., Warrington, R., Bishop, D., Chowdhury, U., Lau, J., Mandy, W., et al. (2004). The developmental, dimensional and diagnostic interview (3di): A novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 548–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X. Y., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Lely, H. K. J., & Howard, D. (1993). Specifically language impaired children: Linguistic impairment or short-term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1193–1207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Abrahams, B. S., Winchester, L., Nicod, J., Groszer, M., et al. (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 2337–2345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Fourth edition (UK). London: Pearson Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 319–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J. O., Bishop, D. V. M., Ang, Q. W., Pennel, C. E., & Fisher, S. E. (2011). CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 451–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. M., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008a). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D., Happe, F., & Jarrold, C. (2008b). Intact inner speech use in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a short-term memory task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(1), 51–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. M., & Lind, S. E. (2013). Comorbidity and diagnosis of developmental disorders. In C. R. Marshall (Ed.), Current Issues in Developmental Disorders (pp. 19–45). London: Psychology Press.

  • Williams, D. M., Bowler, D. M., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inner speech is used to mediate short-term memory, but not planning, among intellectually high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 225–239.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The third author was funded by a Leverhulme Early Career Fellowship (ECF/2008/0469). An oral presentation of the results of this paper was delivered by the first author at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada, April 2011. We would like to thank the children and schools who participated in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to David Williams or Chloë Marshall.

Additional information

This study was conducted during a City University Research Fellowship awarded to the first author.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 4.

Table 4 Non-word stimuli

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Williams, D., Payne, H. & Marshall, C. Non-word Repetition Impairment in Autism and Specific Language Impairment: Evidence for Distinct Underlying Cognitive Causes. J Autism Dev Disord 43, 404–417 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1579-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1579-8

Keywords

Navigation