Skip to main content
Log in

Should social pragmatic communication disorder be included in DSM-5? On uncertainties, pragmatic considerations, and the psychiatric kind debate

  • Paper in Philosophy of Science in Practice
  • Published:
European Journal for Philosophy of Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract 

In this paper, we want to take a critical stance towards Tsou’s recent proposal that a neuro-oriented version of the homeostatic property cluster kind model (MPCK) should be an ideal for the DSM. Our strategy will be to discuss the creation of the Social Pragmatic Communication Disorder (SPCD) in DSM-5 to show the limits of MPCK as an ideal for the next DSM deliberations over a set of diagnoses revisions. We argue that an ideal model for the DSM should address the issues of deep uncertainties and pragmatic considerations to be useful in this context.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It should be noted that there are convincing critiques of the idea that autism spectrum is a disorder (see for instance Fenton and Krahn (2007) and Chapman (2021)). This is why in this paper, when possible, we will distance ourselves from the pathologizing view of autism promoted by the DSM. When analyzing the conception of autism presented in the DSM we will stick to its language and conceptualization for the sake of clarity, but we do not endorse this view.

  2. For Tsou, this is not to exclude the influence of social and cultural factors in the classification of mental disorders. While social and cultural mechanisms can explain the different expressions of mental disorders, biological mechanisms explain the presence of their core features (e.g., psychosis, depressed mood). Moreover, Tsou acknowledges that for some conditions to count as mental disorders in the DSM, they need to be associated with harmful consequences. However, this would not negate the fact that the DSM should classify mental disorders in relation to their neurobiological causal mechanisms. Only categories that are sustained by neurobiological mechanisms that cause harm would count as real mental disorders.

  3. Tsou’s account acknowledges the role of pragmatic considerations so long as they target causally distinctive biological kinds (Tsou 2022).

  4. This change was motivated mainly by the apparent lack of clinical utility of Rett syndrome as a manifestation of autism (see e.g., Towbin 2005; Walker et al., 2004). The conceptualization of autism as a spectrum was seen as the best solution to overcome issues regarding the different manifestations of autism (see e.g., Volkmar 2009).

  5. For more discussion on some of these views, see Amoretti, Lalumera and Serpico 2021

  6. It should be noted that according to a more recent study (Saul et al., 2021), it seems that only a very small percentage of children would have “pure” social communication difficulties only (below 1%). In the clinics, these profiles are not isolated, and usually come with language difficulties or autistic traits. We could therefore view SPCD as a crossover between these two diagnoses rather than a discrete entity, which would give credence to positions B and C described above. The position that social communication difficulties are a transdiagnostic symptom (D) does not consider the context of the prevalence of social difficulties, which appear to be even more pronounced in children with language impairments or in the autism spectrum than in the children population with emotional regulation or behavioural difficulties. However, more research is still needed.

  7. An institutional aspect to consider here is that even if one of the goals of the DSM-5 revision process was to ground changes on systematic evidence reviews, there was no strong requirement for the Work Groups to do so. As pointed out by Kendler and Solomon (2016), two main problems can be attributed to the SRC. First, it was created in 2009, after the Work Groups have begun to work on change proposals. Second, the SRC didn’t have the resources needed to establish an effective scientific literature review (see also Kendler 2013). Widiger and Crego (2015, see also Widiger et al., 2015) have provided evidence that Work Groups were not required to follow the Guidelines, making it a purely advisory document.

  8. We thank an anonymous referee for suggesting this additional example.

References

  • Adams, C., & Gaile, J. (2020). Evaluation of a parent preference-based outcome measure after intensive communication intervention for children with social (pragmatic) communication disorder and high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 105, 103752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, VA

  • Amoretti, M. C., & Lalumera, E. (2021). Wherein is the concept of disease normative? From weak normativity to value-conscious naturalism. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 1–14.

  • Amoretti, M. C., Lalumera, E., & Serpico, D. (2021). The DSM-5 introduction of the Social (Pragmatic) Communication Disorder as a new mental disorder: A philosophical review. History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences, 43(4), 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, G., & Norbury, C. F. (2016). Social (pragmatic) communication disorders and autism spectrum disorder. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 101(8), 745–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebee, H., & Sabbarton-Leary, N. (2010). Are psychiatric kinds real? European Journal of Analytic Philosophy, 6(1), 11–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1989). Autism, Asperger’s Syndrome and semantic-pragmatic disorder: Where are the boundaries? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 24(2), 107–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the children’s communication checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 39, 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: phenomimicry or shared etiology?. Behavior Genetics, 40, 618–629.

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardized diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 917–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., & Thompson, P. A. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE : A multinational and muldisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development : Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1999). Pragmatic language impairment without autism. Autism, 3, 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R. (1991). Realism, anti-foundationalism and the enthusiasm for natural kinds. Philosophical Studies, 61(1), 127–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R. (1999). Homeostasis, species, and higher taxa. Species: New interdisciplinary essays, 141, 185.

  • Brukner-Wertman, Y., Laor, N., & Golan, O. (2016). Social (pragmatic) communication disorder and its relation to the autism spectrum: Dilemmas arising from the DSM-5 classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(8), 2821–2829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bueter, A. (2019). Epistemic injustice and psychiatric classification. Philosophy of Science, 86(5), 1064–1074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, R. (2021). Neurodiversity and the social ecology of mental functions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 1360–1372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Mok, P. L., Pickles, A., & Durkin, K. (2013). Adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI): Strengths and difficulties in social, emotional and behavioral functioning. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(11), 4161–4169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. (2016). First Do No Harm?: What Role Should Considerations of Potential Harm Play in Revising the DSM? Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 23(2), 103–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Drew, A., & Wheelwright, S. (1999). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age : Stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 719–732.

  • Cratsley, K. (2019). The ethical and empirical status of dimensional diagnosis: Implications for public mental health? Neuroethics, 12(2), 183–199.

  • Crichton, P., Havi, C., & Kidd, I. J. (2017). Epistemic Injustice in Psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry Bulletin, 41(2), 65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenton, A., &Krahn, T. (2007). Autism, Neurodiversity, and Equality beyond the “Normal.” Journal of Ethics in Mental Health, 2(2).

  • Frances, A. J., & Widiger, T. (2012). Psychiatric diagnosis: Lessons from the DSM-IV past and cautions for the DSM-5 future. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 109–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gagné-Julien, A. M. (2021). Wrongful medicalization and epistemic injustice in psychiatry: The case of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. European Journal of Analytic Philosophy, 17(2), 4–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon, L., Mottron, L., & Joanette, Y. (1997). Questioning the Validity of the Semantic-Pragmatic Syndrome Diagnosis. Autism, 1(1), 37–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gonçalves, A. M. N., Dantas, C. D. R., & Banzato, C. E. (2016). Values and DSM-5: Looking at the debate on attenuated psychosis syndrome. BMC Medical Ethics, 17(1), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P. E. (1999). Squaring the Circle: Natural Kinds with Historical Essences. In R. A. Wilson (Ed.), Species: New Interdisciplinary Essays (pp. 209–228). The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1218–1220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (2011). Criteria, categories, and continua: Autism and related disorders in DSM-5. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(6), 540–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartner, D. F., & Theurer, K. L. (2018). Psychiatry should not seek mechanisms of disorder. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 38(4), 189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasin, D., & Paykin, A. (1998). Dependence symptoms but no diagnosis: Diagnostic “orphans” in a community sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 50(1), 19–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasin, D. S., O'Brien, C. P., Auriacombe, M., Borges, G., Bucholz, K., Budney, A., Grant, B. F. (2013). DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorders: Recommendations and rationale. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 834–851.

  • Kerbeshian, J., Burd, L., & Fisher, W. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome : To be or not to be ? The British Journal of Psychiatry, 156(5), 721–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y. S., Fombonne, E., Koh, Y., Kim, S., Cheon, K., & Leventhal, B. L. (2014). Developmental disorder and DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder prevalence in an epidemiologic sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(5), 500–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S. (1980). The nosologic validity of paranoia (simple delusional disorder). A Review. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 699–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K., Kupfer, D., Narrow, W., Phillips, K., & Fawcett, J. (2009). Guidelines for making changes to DSM-V. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Kendler, K. S., Zachar, P., & Craver, C. (2011). What kinds of things are psychiatric disorders? Psychological Medicine, 41(6), 1143–1150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S. (2012). The dappled nature of causes of psychiatric illness: Replacing the organic– functional/ hardware– software dichotomy with empirically based pluralism. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 377–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S. (2013). A history of the DSM-5 Scientific Review Committee. Psychological Medicine, 43(9), 1793–1800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S., & Solomon, M. (2016). Expert consensus v. evidence-based approaches in the revision of the DSM. Psychological Medicine, 46(11), 2255–2262.

  • Kincaid, H., & Sullivan, J. A. (Eds.). (2014). Classifying psychopathology: Mental kinds and natural kinds. MIT Press.

  • Kupfer, D. J., Regier, D. A., & Kuhl, E. A. (2008). On the road to DSM-V and ICD-11. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 258(5), 2–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, G., & Dockrell, J. E. (2012). Longitudinal patterns of behavioral, emotional, and social difficulties and self-concepts in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 43(4), 445–460.

  • Mandy, W., Wang, A., Lee, I., & Skuse, D. (2017). Evaluating social (pragmatic) communication disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1166–1175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, D. (2006). Psychiatry in the scientific image. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A. J., Walter, J., Stein, J. F., Talcott, J. B., & Monaco A. P. (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading-and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.

  • Norbury, C. F. (2014). Practitioner Review: Social (pragmatic) communication disorder conceptualization, evidence and clinical implications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(3), 204–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parnas, J., Nordgaard, J., & Varga, S. (2010). The concept of psychosis: A clinical and theoretical analysis. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 7(2), 32–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosologic considerations. In: U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language, reading and spelling. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Regier, D. A., Narrow, W. E., Kuhl, E. A., Kupfer, D. J., & American Psychopathological Association. (2010). The conceptual evolution of DSM-5. American Psychiatric Pub.

  • Reisinger, L. M., Cornish, K. M. & Fombonne, E. (2011). Diagnostic differentiation of autism spectrum disorders and pragmatic language impairment. 41. 12. 1694–1704.

  • Robins, E., & Guze, S. B. (1970). Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: Its application to schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 983–987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (2014) Addressing the issue of fractionation in autism spectrum disorder: A commentary on Brunsdon and Happé, Frazier, Hobson, and Mandy et al. Autism, 18,55-57

  • Sadler, J. Z. (2015). Ethics and values in diagnosing and classifying psychopathology. In J. Z. Sadler, W. (C. W.) Van Staden, & K. W. M. Fulford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psychiatric ethics (pp. 1045–1068). Oxford University Press.

  • Saul, J., Griffiths, S. L., & Norbury, C. (2021). Prevalence and functional impact of social (pragmatic) communication disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(3), 376–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shields, J. R., Rosemary, V., Broks, P., & Adrian, S. (1996). Social cognition in developmental language disorders and high-level autism. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 38(6), 487–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M. (2017). On the appearance and disappearance of Asperger’s syndrome. Philosophical Issues in Psychiatry IV: Psychiatric Nosology, 14.

  • Solomon, M., & Kendler, K. S. (2021). The Problem of Aggregating Validators for Psychiatric Disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 209(1), 9–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, D. (2017). Obsessive-compulsive and related disorders in DSM-5, ICD-11, and RDoC: Conceptual questions and practical solutions”. In Philosophical Issues in Psychiatry IV: Psychiatric Nosology, (pp. 55–69).

  • Sturm, A., Rozenman, M., Chang, S., McGough, J. J., & James, T. (2018). Are the components of social reciprocity transdiagnostic across pediatric neurodevelopmental disorders? Evidence for common and disorder-specific social impairments. Psychiatry Research, 264, 119–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swineford, L. B., Thurm, A., Baird, G., Wetherby, A. M., & Swedo, S. (2014). Social (pragmatic) communication disorder: A research review of this new DSM-5 diagnostic category. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6(1), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabb, K. (2019). Philosophy of psychiatry after diagnostic kinds. Synthese, 196(6), 2177–2195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, L. J., & Whitehouse, A. J. (2016). Autism spectrum disorder, language disorder, and social (pragmatic) communication disorder: Overlaps, distinguishing features, and clinical implications. Australian Psychologist, 51(4), 287–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tekin, Ş. (2014). Psychiatric taxonomy: At the crossroads of science and ethics. Journal of Medical Ethics, 40(8), 513–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tekin, Ş. (2016). Are mental disorders natural kinds?: A plea for a new approach to intervention in psychiatry. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 23(2), 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towbin, K. E. (2005). Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 165–200). Wiley.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tsou, J. Y. (2012). Intervention, causal reasoning, and the neurobiology of mental disorders: Pharmacological drugs as experimental instruments. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part c: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 43(2), 542–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsou, J. Y. (2013). Depression and suicide are natural kinds: Implications for physician-assisted suicide. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 36(5–6), 461–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsou, J. Y. (2015). DSM-5 and psychiatry’s second revolution: Descriptive vs. theoretical approaches to psychiatric classification. In Demazeux, S. and P. Singy, The DSM-5 in perspective (pp. 43–62). Dordrecht: Springer.

  • Tsou, J. Y. (2016). Natural kinds, psychiatric classification and the history of the DSM. History of Psychiatry, 27(4), 406–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsou, J. (2021). Philosophy of Psychiatry (Elements in the Philosophy of Science). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsou, J. Y. (2022). Biological Essentialism, Projectable Human Kinds, and Psychiatric Classification. Philosophy of Science, 1–21.

  • Varga, S. (2018). “Relaxed” natural kinds and psychiatric classification. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part c: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 72, 49–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F., State, M., & Klin, A. (2009). Autism and autism spectrum disorders: Diagnostic issues for the coming decade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1–2), 108–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeff, B. (2012). What is this thing called autism? A critical analysis of the tenacious search for autism’s essence. BioSocieties, 7(4), 410–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, D. R., Thompson, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., Goldberg, J., Bryson, S. E., et al. (2004). Specifying PDD-NOS: A comparison of PDD-NOS, Asperger syndrome, and autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 172–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J., Watt, H. J., Line, E. A., & Bishop, D. V. (2009). Adult psychosocial outcomes of children with specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(4), 511–528.

  • Widiger, T. A., & Crego, C. (2015). Process and content of DSM-5. Psychopathology Review, 2(1), 162–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widiger, T. A., Crego, C., & Oltmanns, J. R. (2015). The validation of a classification of psychopathology. Psychological Inquiry, 26(3), 272–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yager, J., & Mcintyre, J. S. (2014). DSM-5 Clinical and Public Health Committee: Challenges and considerations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(2), 142–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zachar, P. (2000). Psychiatric disorders are not natural kinds. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 7(3), 167–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zachar, P. (2002). The practical kinds model as a pragmatist theory of classification. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 9(3), 219–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zachar, P., & Kendler, K. S. (2007). Psychiatric disorders: A conceptual taxonomy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(4), 557–565.

  • Zachar, P., Regier, D. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2019). The aspirations for a paradigm shift in DSM-5: An oral history. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 207(9), 778–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Peter Zachar, Elisabetta Lalumera, Sarah Arnaud, Karine Larose and Martin St-André for helpful comments with previous versions of this paper.

Funding

This study was funded by Fonds de Recherche du Québec – Société et Culture – Postdoctoral fellowship.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Both authors had the idea for the paper, literature search was performed by both authors (Anne-Marie Gagné-Julien focused on the philosophical literature and Andréanne Bérubé focused on the scientific literature. Both authors performed the analysis, and Anne-Marie Gagné-Julien critically revised the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne-Marie Gagné-Julien.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

Does not apply.

Financial or non-financial interests

The authors declare they have no financial interests or non-financial interests.

Informed consent

Does not apply.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gagné-Julien, AM., Bérubé, A. Should social pragmatic communication disorder be included in DSM-5? On uncertainties, pragmatic considerations, and the psychiatric kind debate. Euro Jnl Phil Sci 13, 29 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13194-023-00535-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13194-023-00535-8

Keywords

Navigation