Introduction

Mental health problems are prevalent among people living with HIV (PLWH) [1, 2]. It is evident that such problems were positively associated with risk behaviors [3] and disease progression [1, 2], and negatively associated with self-care and service seeking behaviors [4, 5]. It also increased the likelihood of HIV transmissions [6] among PLWH. A large segment of PLWH consists of men who have sex with men (MSM). For instance, 40% of the 437,000 PLWH reported in China in 2013 were MSM [7].

HIV-positive MSM are highly vulnerable to mental health problems. The World Health Organization pointed out that MSM in general showed higher prevalence of psychological problems but lower prevalence of mental health service utilization than other men [8]. As expected, the prevalence of psychological problems was even higher among HIV-positive MSM than MSM and PLWH in general [9], as HIV-positive MSM face numerous stressors and double stigma toward both PLWH and MSM [10]. The prevalence of suicidal ideation among HIV-positive MSM ranged from 28 to 59% in the U.S. [11, 12] and was 31% in China [13], while that of depression ranged from 42.9 to 55.8% in China [13, 14]. However, related preventive and treatment services targeting HIV-positive MSM are warranted but not readily available [15,16,17].

For various reasons, mental health issues among newly diagnosed HIV-positive MSM (NHMSM) require special attention. First, NHMSM are taking up a new disease identity. Second, they are in the initial phase of adjustment and the initial coping strategies adopted may have lasting effects on their mental and physical health outcomes [18]. Third, they are forming their perceptions related to HIV; such evolving perceptions may have lasting effects on mental health. Fourth, it is a critical time period for the development of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [18] and post-traumatic growth (PTG). Thus, early mental health interventions targeting NHMSM may have significant long-term effects.

Despite the importance, there is a dearth of studies in literature investigating mental health problems among NHMSM. There are seven quantitative studies and two qualitative studies investigating mental health of NHMSM [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27]. Six of them were conducted in China [20,21,22,23, 26, 27], two studies were conducted in the U.S. [19, 24], and one was conducted in Australia [25]. They were based on seven independent samples [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27]. Four studies reported the prevalence of depression (22.8–36% in mainland China and 22.2% in Australia) and/or anxiety (28.5–42.0% in mainland China) [21,22,23, 25]. One of these studies investigated links between understandings about adversity and different outcomes, including PTG [20]. None of these studies involved illness perception.

While traditional psychology focuses on abnormality, positive psychology emphasizes the positive aspects of mental health and personal growth [28]. PTG, an important topic in positive psychology, is defined as positive changes as a result of struggling with a traumatic event [29]. Individuals who experience PTG would perceive benefits in three broad domains: changes in self-perception (e.g., greater sense of personal strength), changes in interpersonal relations (e.g., more intimate relationships with others), and changes in their philosophy of life (e.g., recognition of new possibilities or paths for one’s life and spiritual development) [30]. PTG can co-exist with PTSD [31,32,33,34,35]. It was negatively associated with suicidal ideation [35, 36] and psychological distress [36,37,38], and positively associated with optimism [39], positive affect, self-efficacy, resilience and social support [40,41,42,43]. One meta-analysis reported that PTG improved physical and mental health among cancer patients and PLWH [29]. Interventions can effectively promote PTG [44] and hence potentially alleviate mental health problems indirectly. Validated measurements [45, 46] are available to assess PTG, and have been applied to Chinese survivors of accidental injury, cancer, coronary heart diseases, and SARS [31, 35, 37, 38, 40, 41, 43]. To our knowledge, only two studies have investigated PTG among PLWH living in China [47, 48].

How patients perceive illnesses may affect the development of PTG. The processing of trauma-related cognitions (e.g., appraisal, adaptation, and meaning-making) is a potential mechanism for formation of PTG [30, 49]. Illness perception is defined as patients’ beliefs and expectations about an illness or related somatic symptoms [50]. It includes cognitive representation and emotional representation, which are the key components of the Common Sense Model, also known as the Illness Perception Model or Self-regulatory Model [51, 52]. The model (Fig. 1) postulates that illness stimuli caused by a health threat (e.g., HIV-positive diagnosis) would induce cognitive and emotional representations. Such representations would influence coping strategies to determine illness outcomes (e.g., disease status) and emotional outcomes (e.g., emotional distress), respectively, in two parallel and inter-related processes. Through appraisals of such health-related outcomes, the individual would also update the illness stimuli of the health threat. The model (part or full) has been applied to study diseases such as cardiovascular, metabolic and cancer [53], as well as HIV/AIDS [54,55,56].

Fig. 1
figure 1

The Common Sense Model

It is not our purpose to test the Common Sense Model, which is beyond the scope of the study. The model is an important one as it provides researchers with a strong rationale to study important potential associations between cognitive/emotional representations and health-related outcomes. Empirically, our study and many others have investigated such associations without testing the mediating effects of coping strategies and the part on appraisal, nor testing the entire model. Examples of these studies included those that focused on health-related outcomes such as functional adaptation, help-seeking behaviors, and adherence to medical recommendations [55, 57,58,59]. A meta-analysis further showed that the two types of cognitive representations were associated with depression, anxiety, distress, worry, negative affect and intrusion [60].

A literature search on PubMed using keywords “illness perception” and “China” found 22 published papers that applied illness perception to understand various health-related outcomes [61, 62]. Although such studies did not focus on PLWH in China, some of them targeted PLWH of other countries. To our knowledge, no study has targeted NHMSM in any country. Like PTG, illness perception can be modified by interventions [63,64,65]. To our knowledge, no study has investigated PTG among NHMSM and its relationship with illness perception.

As seen from Fig. 1, the self-regulation adaptation consists of two partially parallel processes that involve cognitive and emotional representations, and they interact with each other to determine one’s coping strategies and health-related outcomes [66]. It implies that strong negative emotional representation may compromise the positive impact and/or inflate the negative impact of cognitive representation on PTG. To our knowledge, no study has tested such a moderation hypothesis. Furthermore, the cognitive theory of emotions posits that cognitive processes influence emotional responses [67]. Specifically, cognitive beliefs about a disease can affect one’s emotional response towards the disease [67]. The Common Sense Model [51, 52], supported by empirical findings, also suggests that constructs of cognitive representation and emotional representation are significantly correlated with each other [68,69,70]. It is possible that the association between cognitive representation and PTG may be mediated via emotional representation. Although several studies suggested that emotional representation and cognitive representation showed independent impact on health outcomes such as quality of life, emotion and stress [71,72,73], no study has tested this mediation hypothesis explicitly. For the first time, the mediation and moderation hypotheses were tested in this study.

We investigated associations between the two constructs of illness perception (cognitive representation and emotional representation) and PTG among NHMSM. Following other studies, we defined NHMSM as MSM who were confirmed as HIV-positive during the last 12 months [74,75,76,77,78,79]. We further tested the two hypotheses, that emotional representation would interact with cognitive representation to affect PTG, and that emotional representation would mediate the associations between constructs of cognitive representation and PTG.

Method

Study Design and Data Collection

Inclusion criteria were (1) Chinese men aged 16 or above, (2) confirmed as HIV-positive in the last 12 months, and (3) anal intercourse with at least one man in the last year. We collaborated with the largest non-governmental organization (NGO) providing HIV prevention and care services to PLWH and MSM in Chengdu, Sichuan province, China, which has a population size of about 10 million. There were 70,000 MSM living in Chengdu (2011) [80], and their HIV prevalence was 15.0% in 2010 [81] and 26.2% in 2013 [82].

Recruitment procedures were as follows: (1) Six staff of the NGO went through the service records and contacted all the NGO’s service users who were HIV-positive MSM through phone calls or social media or during their visits, or participants’ referrals. (2) During the contact, the staff asked a few screening questions to identify NHMSM who were eligible to join the study (n = 282), according to the inclusion criteria. (3) These 282 prospective participants were briefed about the study, and were informed that refusals would not affect their right to use any services and they could quit any time without being questioned. (4) With written informed consent, anonymous face-to-face interviews were conducted in a private room at the NGO. (5) Out of the 282 NHMSM contacted by the staff, 57 (20.2%) were refusals and 225 (79.8%) consented and completed the study. (6) Participants received a monetary compensation (50 RMB or 7.8 USD) for their time spent.

Measurements

Background Variables

Background information collected included socio-demographic characteristics, permanent resident status of Chengdu, duration of stay in Chengdu, and sexual orientation. HIV-related information [date of HIV diagnosis, CD4 count found in the last episode of testing, and use of antiretroviral treatment (ART)] was recorded. The background characteristics of the sample are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1 Background characteristics of the sample (N = 225)

PTG

The PTG scale was based on Milam’s [39] work targeting PLWH. Participants were asked to report whether they had experienced positive or negative changes with regards to various dimensions of their life. The response categories were from 1 (highly negative change) to 5 (highly positive change), with 3 = no change [39]. After consulting some NHMSM, two (out of 11) items on spirituality were omitted due to low cultural relevance. Sample items included “appreciation for the value of your own life” and “your own inner strength” (see Table 2 for all items). In this study, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) extracted one factor that explained 64.1% of the variance [KMO test = 0.84; Bartlett’s test = 933.8 (p < 0.001)]. All items were used in data analyses. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87.

Table 2 Item responses of the Post-traumatic growth scale (n = 225)

Illness Perception

The revised Illness Perception Questionnaire for HIV (IPQ-R-HIV) was used in this study. The original scale measured one dimension of emotional representation and seven dimensions of cognitive representation [identity, timeline acute/chronic, consequences, personal control, treatment control, illness coherence (perceived understanding of the illness), and timeline cyclical] [83]. We omitted the timeline-cyclical subscale as HIV infection was a widely known chronic condition. An additional dimension of cognitive representation (i.e., causal attribution) was assessed by seven items adapted from Reynolds’ work [58]; they measured six subscales [god’s punishment (two items), carelessness, being deceived, unaware of the risk, chance/luck, and substance use]. All items were assessed with responses from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The ER subscale consisted of six items that assessed negative emotions towards HIV (e.g., “Having HIV makes me feel angry”, “Having HIV makes me feel afraid”, and “When I think about having HIV I get upset). The number of items for each of the subscales is listed in Table 3. All the subscales used in this study were translated into Chinese and back-translated into English by two bilingual health workers to ensure linguistic equivalence. Feedback obtained from a pilot study of 11 HIV-positive MSM was used to finalize the questionnaire.

Table 3 Means and standard deviation of sub-scales of the revised illness perception questionnaire for HIV

Statistical Analysis

Multiple linear regression models were fit using the PTG as the dependent variable and IPQ-R-HIV subscales as the independent variables, adjusted for background variables having p < 0.10 in the univariate analysis. All variables with p < 0.10 obtained in such an analysis were used as candidates for fitting a forward stepwise multiple linear regression model (entry: p < 0.10; removal: p > 0.20). The SPSS for Windows software package (version 16.0) was used for statistical analyses; p < 0.05 was taken as statistically significant and 0.05 < p < 0.10 was considered marginally statistically significant.

To test the moderation hypothesis, product terms (emotional representation × a subscale of cognitive representation) were created and added to the main-effect model to assess improvement in goodness-of-fit (by assessing changes in F-values) of the hierarchical models involved. According to Baron and Kenny [84], emotional representation mediates the association between a particular subscale of cognitive representation and PTG, only if (1) it is associated with both PTG scale and the CR subscale, and (2) the subscale of cognitive representation is associated with PTG. For each subscale of cognitive representation fulfilling such requirements, the subscale of emotional representation was added to the model containing the cognitive representation subscale of interest as the independent variable and PTG as the dependent variable (adjusted for significant background variables). Full or partial mediation effect would have been detected if the original significant regression coefficient became non-significant or remained significant but substantially diminished, respectively.

Results

Background Characteristics

Of the 225 participants, about half (53.8%) were aged 30 or below (mean = 32.2; SD = 10.5; range 17–71 years), possessed a college degree (44.4%), were full time workers (62.2%), and had a monthly income > 000 RMB (about 312 USD) (48%). Over 2/3 (69.8%) had lived in Chengdu for ≥2 years but only 36% possessed permanent residency status. About 3/4 (72.0%) self-reported as homosexuals. Two-thirds (66.7%) were currently single or divorced and 1/3 (33.3%) were currently married or cohabiting with a woman. About 2/3 (61.8%) had their HIV diagnosed during the last 6 months (mean duration = 5.64 months; SD = 3.76 months). About 1/3 (31.1%) obtained a CD4 count <350 cells/ml (last episode of testing); 20.4% were currently on ART.

Attributes of PTG and IPQ-R-HIV

Over 40% endorsed a positive or strongly positive change regarding the PTG scale items that were related to closeness with family/friends, compassion for others, priority in life and importance in life; 25–40% showed such positive changes regarding willingness to express emotions, ability in handling difficulties, involvement in interesting things, and inner strength (see Table 2). There were also 6.2–48.9% giving item responses reflecting negative changes. The overall mean PTG scale score was 27.68 (SD = 7.31, range 9–45). Descriptive statistics of the IPQ-R-HIV subscales are summarized in Table 3.

Factors of PTG

Negative associations were found between age >30 years old and PTG (p = 0.008), and between having lived in Chengdu and PTG (p = 0.078) (see Table 4). The results of the bivariate analysis involving emotional representation and cognitive representation are summarized in Table 4. The Emotional Representation subscale and nine out of the 12 cognitive representation subscales showed statistically significant correlations with PTG. The absolute values of the significant correlation coefficients ranged from 0.13 to 0.51 (see Table 5). The three non-significant cognitive representation variables were attrition to deception, lack of awareness and attrition to substance use (r ranged from 0.01 to 0.08; p > 0.05).

Table 4 Associations between background variables and post-traumatic growth
Table 5 Associations between subscales of the revised illness perception questionnaire for HIV and post-traumatic growth

In the main adjusted multiple linear regression analysis (also shown in Table 4), negatively associated factors of PTG included the Emotional Representation subscale (β = −0.49) and five cognitive representation subscales [timeline (β = −0.32), consequence (β = −0.37), identity (β = −0.20), attribution to god’s punishment/will (β = −0.23), and attribution to chance/luck (β = −0.13)]; positive associations involved four cognitive representation subscales [treatment control (β = 0.48), illness coherence (β = 0.32), personal control (β = 0.51) and attribution to carelessness (β = 0.15)] (see Table 5).

Using those variables that were significant in the adjusted analysis as candidates, a forward stepwise multiple regression model identified three significant subscales that were independently associated with PTG: emotional representation (β = −0.63), identity (β = −0.82); and attribution to carelessness (β = 0.99) (entry: p < 0.10, removal: p > 0.20; R square = 0.553). To avoid repetition, these statistics are not shown in the tables.

Testing Moderating Effects

No significant moderation effect was detected as none of the interaction terms involving the subscales of cognitive representation and Emotional Representation subscale was statistically significant (data were not shown in the tables).

Testing Mediation Effects

Since three subscales of cognitive representation that were significantly associated with PTG were not significantly correlated with emotional representation [personal control (Spearman r = −0.115, p = 0.085), identity (Spearman r = 0.126, p = 0.060) and attrition to chance/luck (Spearman r = 0.032, p = 0.636)], the mediation requirements were not all fulfilled and hence ER could not mediate their associations with PTG. The other six subscales of cognitive representation (i.e., illness coherence, timeline, consequence, attribution to god’s punishment/will, and attribution to chance/luck) were significantly associated with emotional representation (Spearman correlation coefficients ranged from −0.46 to 0.75, p < 0.05); their mediation hypotheses were tested in Table 5. The results show that five out of these six cognitive representation subscales became statistically non-significant after adjusting for both emotional representation and the significant background variables; their associations with PTG were hence fully mediated by emotional representation. Partial mediation via ER was detected for the association between treatment control and PTG, as the β of treatment control remained statistically significant but diminished from 0.21 to 0.13 after adding emotional representation to the model (Table 6).

Table 6 Testing mediation effect of emotional representation for the associations between cognitive representation and post-traumatic growth

Discussion

PTG has no cut-off point as it is a spectrum reflecting extent of changes. Participants showed multi-facet PTG (e.g., closeness with family/friends, compassion with others, priority and importance in life). It is important to understand that such growth might have already occurred within the first half and maintained in the second half of the first year since HIV diagnosis, as duration since HIV diagnosis (i.e., <6 vs ≥6 months) was not significantly associated with PTG. Hence, interventions fostering PTG among NHMSM should start early. Longitudinal studies are required to test this conjecture and track changes in level of PTG among NHMSM over time

We found that younger age (≤30 years old) was associated with PTG. Segmentation by age should be considered when designing related interventions, as those targeting younger NHMSM may be more effective in promoting PTG. About two-thirds (64%) of the participants were internal migrants (i.e., no permanent residency status of Chengdu). Although permanent residency status of a city was associated with depression and anxiety among HIV-positive MSM [85], it was not significantly associated with PTG among our sampled NHMSM.

As contextual background, according to the national guideline used at the time when the study was conducted [86], PLWH with CD4 level <350 cells/ml should be given free ART. However, only 2/3 of our participants fulfilling that condition were taking up ART. This might be partially related to the policy that PLWH need to receive ART in their hometowns while many participants were internal migrants and were hence ineligible to receive free ART. In a previous study, availability of ART was significantly associated with depression among NHMSM [26]. In contrast, this study showed that treatment status was not significantly associated with PTG. Since not all participants showed low CD4 counts (<350 cells/ml), some of those who were not on treatment were having higher CD4 counts and hence better health conditions. The treatment variable used in this study thus did not fully represent an unmet demand, although factors of PTG might be different from those of psychological problems [87]. Such mixture of CD4 levels and treatment eligibility might partially explain the non-significance of the association between treatment status and PTG. Also, PTG might be less affected by unfavorable conditions as positive psychology is involved. Further meta-analysis is warranted to test this contention.

The hypotheses that cognitive/emotional representation correlate with PTG were mostly supported by our data. In the adjusted analysis, constructs of cognitive representation including treatment control, personal control and illness coherence were positively associated with PTG. As treatment control involves questions about perceptions on efficacy of ART, it is applicable to all participants, disregarding their treatment status. Personal control may increase self-efficacy and treatment control may increase level of hopefulness, both self-efficacy and hopefulness are protective against mental health problems [88, 89]. To make use of such findings for promotion of PTG among NHMSM, we should enhance sense of personal control and treatment control by affirming beliefs on effectiveness and importance of self-care and ART. Illness coherence refers to the perceived level of understanding of the disease [83]. Hence, relevance of technical concepts such as CD4, viral load and viral suppression, drug adherence, drug resistance, treatment effectiveness, side effects, curability and other issues should be explained to NHMSM in lay languages.

Furthermore, five constructs of cognitive representation were negatively associated with PTG (time-line, consequence, identity, attribution to god’s punishment/will, and attribution to chance). Such findings are also implicative. Whilst we cannot deny the negative consequences of HIV, we should persuade NHMSM through interventions that PLWH can live a reasonably normal life given advancements in ART, and guide them to recognize and appreciate some possible positive consequences, such as those that they have acknowledged. Interventions should help participants to convince themselves that HIV is a chronic disease which is caused by a bio-behavioral process of viral infection, and assist them to accept HIV as part of their daily life, and enhance positive coping. To counteract the negative association between identity (number of symptoms self-perceived as being caused by HIV) and PTG, we should clarify that some of those symptoms that they reported might not have been caused by HIV. It is also interesting to know that attribution to god’s punishment/will and chance might diminish PTG. A study showed that attributions of HIV infection to destiny/higher power and others’ responsibility were associated with lower self-esteem and avoidance coping among Asians [90]. Such external attributions might result in a sense of helplessness and lower control over the disease and its associated consequences, reducing active and positive coping among PLWH [91]. Attribution results from an individual’s social-cognitive processing, and people from collectivistic cultures like Chinese are more likely to attribute adverse events to others, the context, and fate [92]. Future studies may look at cultural differences involving attributions and their impact on PTG.

Our data support the Common Sense Model that, besides the constructs of cognitive representation, emotional representation is an important potential determinant of PTG. In fact, the absolute size of the standardized regression coefficients (β) of emotional representation was found to be the largest one (0.49), followed by that of the cognitive representation construct of consequences (0.37), while those of the other constructs of cognitive representation only ranged from 0.13 to 0.23. Therefore, although nine of the illness perception constructs were significantly correlated with PTG, we found that negative emotional representation and perceived negative HIV/AIDS-related consequences were the two strongest potential barriers inhibiting PTG among NHMSM.

Interventions promoting PTG among PLWH should focus on both types of representations. It is important for health workers to recognize that cognitive and emotional representations have shown to be modifiable [63,64,65], and such modifications can potentially improve PTG among PLWH. Our literature review however, found no study reporting interventions for promotion of PTG among PLWH, although such interventions are warranted.

An important finding is that emotional representation has particular relevance in promoting PTG among PLWH. First, the construct showed the strongest β amongst all illness perception constructs. Second, such a strong association has particular implications on intervention for enhancement of PTG among NHMSM, as emotional representation of NHMSM is newly formed and strong. We should facilitate NHMSM to understand and manage their negative emotions caused by their newly formed HIV status effectively, and to foster corresponding positive coping.

Furthermore, according to the Common Sense Model, interventions for promotion of PTG among NHMSM should foster effective strategies to cope with HIV-related cognitive and emotional representations and related stressors. Such interventions should also enhance social support, especially emotional support, in order to buffer negative emotional representation’s potential compromising effect on PTG [93].

It is not the purpose of this study to investigate the associations between cognitive/emotional representation and mental health problems (e.g., anxiety and depression). Some studies have reported such associations [94,95,96]. Interventions that focused on reduction of anxiety and depression among PLWH have involved various approaches such as cognitive behavioral therapy [97, 98], stress-coping [99], and positive psychology attributes such as resilience and mindfulness [100, 101]. To our knowledge, such mental health interventions targeting PLWH have not explicitly considered modification of specific HIV-related emotional representation, although some of them have focused on improving emotions and moods in general. Future RCT studies may investigate efficacy of interventions based on cognitive and emotional representations in reducing mental health problems such as depression and anxiety among HIV-positive MSM and other types of PLWH. Overall, our findings have shed light on a new direction of research and intervention.

It is a novel finding that out of the seven significant associations between CR subscales and PTG that fulfilled the requirements for testing the hypothesis of emotional representation being a potential mediator, five (i.e., illness coherence, timeline, consequence, attribution to god’s punishment/will, and attribution to chance/luck) of such associations were fully mediated and two (treatment control and identity) were partially mediated by emotional representation. It suggests that some cognitive representation constructs might increase/reduce emotional representation, which may in turn facilitate/prohibit PTG. Thus, interventions based on cognitive representation can potentially promote PTG indirectly through changes in emotional representations. Personal control and attribution to carelessness were associated with PTG but not with emotional representation. Their effects on PTG were hence independent from that of emotional representation, possibly because unlike other constructs of cognitive representation, they are less likely to initiate strong emotions. The moderation hypothesis suggested by the Common Sense Model was however, not supported by our data. To our knowledge, no other study has tested such a moderation hypothesis. Our findings on mediations and moderations highlight new research directions for refining the theory. Future research should confirm the inter-relationships between emotional/cognitive representation and health-related behaviors and outcomes.

Our findings are preliminary in a way, as we only looked at a very special psychological outcome (i.e., PTG). Relationships between cognitive/emotional representation and other important outcomes (e.g., mental health problems, coping style, risk behaviors, healthy life style, service seeking, and drug adherence) should also be investigated to inform interventions and confirm/improve the Common Sense Model. As mentioned, a number of studies have looked at such outcomes in the context of other diseases [55, 57,58,59]. We chose to look at the relationship between emotional/cognitive representation and PTG as most of the studies have looked at negative psychological problems (e.g., depression). Yet, we believe that positive psychology has an important but under-developed role to play in understanding and improving mental health among NHMSM.

The study has some limitations. First, the study was conducted only in one Chinese city; generalization to other cities or the nation should be made with caution. As the sample involved NHMSM, limited generalization can be made to other HIV-positive MSM or non-MSM HIV-positive populations. Second, as there was no sampling frame and random sampling was not feasible, selection bias may have occurred. Third, the 12-month definition of NHMSM was arbitrary although other studies used similar definitions [74,75,76,77,78,79]. Fourth, reporting bias might exist as it is socially desirable to endorse positive changes. Fifth, PTG is a continuous process and our data was unable to capture a dynamic process. The observed relationships were based on cross-sectional data and we cannot establish a causal relationship between illness perception and PTG. Sixth, the cognitive representation variables are correlated with each other and hence would cause co-linearity when entered into the same model. We hence did not fit such a model but instead, fit a stepwise forward regression model that contained variables that were independently associated with PTG. Seventh, we did not test the entire Common Sense Model. Lastly, the mediation hypotheses were also not mentioned in the original model and the theory behind such mediations needs to be further established. We are suggesting a new research direction largely based on empirical results, instead of testing the entire theory.

In conclusion, it is evident that PTG can occur among NHMSM, even within the first 6 or 12 months since one’s HIV diagnosis. Constructs of illness perception (emotional representation and many constructs of cognitive representation) were significantly associated with PTG. The results therefore largely support the Common Sense Model, which has been widely applied to other disease groups. We found that emotional representation did not interact with constructs of cognitive representation to influence PTG. Instead, most of the associations between the constructs of cognitive representation (e.g., illness coherence and attribution to god’s punishment/will) and PTG were fully mediated via emotional representation. Furthermore, the associations between emotional representation/consequences and PTG were especially strong. Therefore, emotional representation plays a very important role in the relationships between illness perception and PTG. However, cognitive representation still has a role to play as some variables of cognitive representation have independent effects on PTG. Furthermore, an interesting and novel conclusion is that cognitive representation’s effect on PTG may largely go through (mediated by) emotional representation. The findings suggest new research directions to understand more about the joint roles of emotional representation and cognitive representation in affecting coping and disease outcomes among NHMSM, other types of PLWH, and even other disease groups. The relationship between cognitive/emotional representation and PTG may vary across socio-demographic groups; future studies may look at such variations. It is also possible to cultivate PTG by changing illness perception through interventions. Pilot studies considering our findings are greatly warranted.