Background

Zoonotic diseases are of major concern for public health and impose a considerable burden on national and global economies [1, 2]. Compared to developed countries, veterinary and human health in developing countries are disproportionately affected by zoonotic pathogens because these countries are often located in the tropics and thus in areas of high pathogen species richness [3]. In addition, community dependence on livestock, high prevalence of bushmeat consumption, lack of food and water safety and security, and frequent interactions with wildlife may expose multiple sections of the human population to zoonotic pathogens [4,5,6,7]. Finally, a typically weak health infrastructure, insufficient training of medical and veterinary health workers, and inefficient cross-sectional collaboration between veterinarians, health practitioners and public health authorities frequently inhibit timely and appropriate diagnosis and treatment of zoonotic diseases [8, 9].

This general setting of potential exposure to zoonotic pathogens combined with limited medical diagnosis facilities and trained personnel in developing countries inevitably assigns substantial responsibility for disease prevention to individuals. Knowledge, attitude and practice studies (KAP) are suitable to assess the extent of knowledge among human populations and document current practices that possibly enhance risks for pathogen infections. Results from KAP studies are crucial to inform and guide public health education programs which attempt to close knowledge gaps and reduce the frequency of practices that potentially favor pathogen transmission [9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Multiple demographic and other human related factors have been hypothesized to influence knowledge of a specific disease, and identifying particular subsets of the human population where knowledge is lacking, could greatly improve the efficacy of education programs [16]. For example, gender has been found to influence knowledge, with males often having greater knowledge about a specific disease [10]. Similarly, being potentially more exposed to a certain pathogen (e.g. living in an area with a high prevalence of a specific pathogen, or keeping animals that may be involved in the transmission of a specific pathogen), and having received formal education may also increase knowledge of a specific disease [10, 15, 17,18,19]. Beyond identifying knowledge gaps and documenting risky practices, KAP studies can further assess the general risk perception regarding different diseases among the local population, particularly if surveys target multiple diseases.

In this study we focused on three zoonotic diseases that are occasionally diagnosed from surveillance systems or research projects in humans living in rural northern Tanzania: anthrax, brucellosis, and rabies. Overall, these diseases may be considered ‘neglected’ diseases and their actual prevalence may be underreported in Tanzania due to challenges associated with integrated disease surveillance and response systems [20, 21], occasional unspecific symptoms of zoonotic diseases (e.g. for brucellosis), and misdiagnoses of zoonotic diseases by medical practitioners [9].

Anthrax (causative agent: Bacillus anthracis) outbreaks have been relatively well documented in our study area. Anthrax outbreaks have caused substantial declines in wild animal populations in the past [22] and the disease is diagnosed among residents of Monduli district [23] and the neighboring Serengeti-Ngorongoro ecosystem [24]. From 2013 to 2016, the reported incidence rate was 7.88 cases / 100,000 persons in the Arusha region of northern Tanzania [23]. Despite being eradicated in some parts of the world, brucellosis (causative agent: Brucella spp.) occurs in wildlife, livestock, and humans of northern Tanzania [9, 12, 19]. In northern Tanzania, Brucella seroprevalence of up to 7.7% in humans and 3–4.6% in livestock has been documented [19]. Rabies (causative agent: rabies virus) circulates in Tanzania (mainly in domestic dog populations but frequent cases are diagnosed among the human and wildlife populations) despite substantial vaccination efforts in several parts of the country [25,26,27,28]. Seroprevalence of rabies in unvaccinated dogs in northern Tanzania can reach 31.6% [29].

We employed a comparative approach to assess knowledge, attitudes and practices regarding these diseases in three districts of northern Tanzania. The study area differs in major land-use forms and contact rates with wildlife species, and captures a variety of ethnic/cultural backgrounds. The main objectives were to assess and compare (1) the knowledge and correlates of knowledge regarding these three diseases, (2) the prevalence of practices that potentially increase infection risk, and (3) risk perceptions towards the three diseases. Finally, (4) we investigated interviewees’ views towards wildlife with respect to veterinary and human health.

Methods

Study area

This interview-based study was carried out in the Karatu, Monduli and Babati districts of northern Tanzania (Fig. 1). The Karatu district is mainly located in the Mbulu highlands [30], which are semi-arid to humid [31]. The Ngorongoro Conservation Area (NCA) and Lake Manyara National Park (LMNP) are bordering village lands in this district which brings several wildlife species near farms of the predominant Iraqw people in the area [30]. The Iraqw are mostly small-holder agriculturalists, cultivating the land with maize, beans, pigeon peas, barley and wheat [32]. The Monduli district is east of the Karatu district and is located in the lowlands of the Great Rift Valley [31]. This area is considered a semi-arid landscape [30] and contains several conservation areas with high densities of wildlife: Lake Manyara National Park (LMNP), Manyara Ranch Conservancy (MR), and the Mto wa Mbu Game-Controlled Area (GCA) [33, 34]. Within this district we mainly sampled people residing in the rural areas around Mto wa Mbu town, which are primarily inhabited by Maasai pastoralists [30]. Within the Babati district, the study took place in villages within the Burunge Wildlife Management Area (WMA). These villages are located close to Tarangire National Park (TNP) and Manyara Ranch Conservancy (MR). The villages are part of the Burunge Wildlife Management Area and are thus in proximity to areas dedicated to wildlife conservation. This part of the Babati district is a semi-arid area dominated by savannah habitats, and home to various ethnicities and agro-pastoral communities [30].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of study area. Locations of household interviews in relation to the major protected areas (NCA = Ngorongoro Conservation Area; LMNP = Lake Manyara National Park; TNP = Tarangire National Park), Lake Manyara (LM) and district boundaries. Households in the village ‘Jangwani’ are not inside LMNP but at its border; the impression that they may be inside the national park may be due to inaccuracies of the protected area shapefile. The inset on the top right indicates the approximate location of the study area within Tanzania. Shapefiles for protected areas and district boundaries are available at: https://protectedplanet.net/country/TZ and https://gadm.org/download_country_v3.html

Interviews

Within each of the three districts, we chose either five (Karatu and Monduli) or four (Babati) villages. We chose villages for their relatively even distribution across the districts, sufficient number of households and accessibility and willingness of village heads to support the study. We conducted interviews using a predetermined and pretested questionnaire over the course of ten days in April 2017 and four days in November 2017. Prior to field work, translators and investigators went through the interview questions to clarify the meaning of each question and translated the questions to Swahili. In addition, we conducted test interviews with residents of Rhotia. We recruited translators from the Cultural Tourism Program in Mto Wa Mbu; all of them had previous research experience with interview-based studies in our study areas. We conducted all interviews in Swahili and translators translated responses immediately to English and responses were recorded in English.

We conducted interviews along 3–5 transects in each village. Approximately every 100 m along each transect, a trained translator (accompanied by one or two investigators) asked a member of a homestead to voluntarily participate in the survey. We only conducted interviews with one person over the age of 18 per household, after gaining verbal consent from the participant. In total, we conducted 388 interviews (Karatu district n = 128; Monduli district n = 114; Babati district n = 146).

We first asked respondents if they were willing to take part in a 45-min survey on knowledge of zoonotic diseases in the area, prior to conducting the interview. We guaranteed participants anonymity as well as the right to stop the interview at any time, in accordance with protocols on rights of human subjects in research. Initially, we asked basic demographic information of the respondent (gender, age, highest level of education, ethnicity, and number of cattle/sheep&goats/dogs owned). We recorded ethnicity as either the predominant ethnicity for each district (i.e. the numerical majority in our sample) or “other” (Karatu: Iraqw vs. other, Monduli: Maasai vs. other, and Babati: Maasai and Arusha combined vs. other). Despite considered different ethnicities, Arusha and Maasai share a common language, a primarily pastoralist lifestyle and many other cultural similarities. We are aware that this may constitute an oversimplification of the ethnic background of interviewees. However, given the diversity of ethnic backgrounds in our study area, we felt that this approach aligned with our major goal (i.e. to identify major determinants of knowledge such as living a primarily pastoralist vs. a primarily farming lifestyle) while ensuring sufficient degrees of freedom in the models.

In line with previous KAP studies [10], we then asked questions that assessed the respondents’ knowledge regarding three zoonotic diseases: anthrax, brucellosis, and rabies. We first asked interviewees if they had heard about this disease. If interviewees had heard about the disease, we asked them whether this disease affect human and/or animals, what kind of symptoms may be present, how this disease can be transmitted, what kind of treatment should be used if a person or animal is infected, if infected persons should consult a medical doctor, and how transmission of this disease can be prevented (Additional file 1: Table S1).

After completing the interviews, responses were jointly assessed and transformed into knowledge points by the same two investigators against the stated criteria outlined in Table 1.

Table 1 Description of symptoms, causes, treatments, and prevention methods for anthrax, brucellosis, and rabies in humans

In addition to questions with regards to knowledge about the three diseases, we asked specific questions about practices that potentially enhance pathogen transmission and about demographic at-risk groups. For example, we asked which demographic groups in a household were responsible for livestock and dog handling. In addition, we asked interviewees about how they prepare or consume milk and meat because raw consumption of these animal products may increase pathogen infection risk (e.g. for brucellosis). Additionally, we asked interviewees whether the co-existence with wildlife generally increases, decreases or does not affect the health of livestock and people. At the end of the interview, we asked respondents to rank each disease in accordance to their perceived danger for human and livestock health; the lowest ranked disease embodied the most perceived danger.

Data analyses

The relative knowledge of each disease was assessed based on the number of obtained knowledge points (i.e. points awarded to answers according to criteria in Table 1) relative to the maximum achievable points for the corresponding disease (anthrax: 10 points; brucellosis: 10 points; rabies: 8 points). ML and RV carefully and jointly read through all answers and assigned knowledge points according to the criteria provided in Table 1.

Because interviewees in the three districts differed considerably in terms of socio-demographic structure, we analyzed data separately for each district and disease. To identify which demographic variables (ethnicity, gender, age, level of education, livestock ownership, dog ownership) were associated with the level of knowledge, we used a generalized linear mixed model with binomial error distribution, using the lme package implemented in the software R [38, 39]. Because the level of knowledge was assessed as proportion (achieved knowledge points / maximum number of achievable points), analyzing the data with linear regression models is not appropriate [40]. To avoid introducing a subjective knowledge threshold, we specified our target variable “knowledge” for each disease as a two-column variable whereas the first column contains the “achieved” points (i.e. successes) and the second column the “missed” points (i.e. failures); columns for successes and failures were combined using the cbind function [40]. Therefore, our model estimates the influence of explanatory variables on the relative knowledge about a specific disease in a logistic regression framework. To account for clustering of interviews (i.e. responses from interviewees from one village may not entirely be independent), we included a random effect for each village. Prior to model fitting, pairwise correlations across explanatory variables were assessed using the corrplot package [41]; since none of the correlation exceeded the 0.7 collinearity threshold (Additional file 2: Figure S2), we included all variables in our models [42]. Due to the high number of a priori hypotheses (gender, age, level of education, ethnicity, livestock ownership for anthrax, and dog ownership for rabies) we first fitted a full model (including all hypothesized variables) and standardized regression coefficients (numeric variables with more than two values were rescaled to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 0.5; binary variables were rescaled to have a mean of 0 and a difference of 1 between their two categories) using the arm package [43]. We then ran all possible permutations of variable combinations (only using additive linear effects) using the MumIn package. Since multiple models had similar model selection support, we model averaged regression coefficients of models within Δ-AICc-values ≤6 using the full average method [44,45,46]. Model selection tables can be found in the electronic appendix (Additional file 3: Table S2). To predict model outcomes, we calculated odds ratios (exponent of regression coefficients) which describe the relative change in knowledge in response to the corresponding explanatory variable i.e., the relative change in knowledge compared to the reference level for categorical variables and the relative change in knowledge when continuous variables change by one unit (note that variables were standardized and the odds ratios thus relate to the mean of the explanatory variable). In line with information-theory we assessed variables based on relative variable importance (computed in the MumIn package) and confidence intervals of regression estimates.

To describe reported practices in relation to zoonotic disease infection risk and at-risk groups, we provide proportions of responses for each district. Proportions were based on the interview sample size in each district (Karatu district n = 128; Monduli district n = 114; Babati district n = 146); in case respondents answered multiple practices or at-risk groups, we created new categories for these answers. We used a Kruskal Wallis anova to test for significant differences in risk perception towards the three diseases and Kendall’s correlation test to assess associations between relative knowledge and risk perception of the three diseases.

Results

Socio-economic characteristics of the interviewees

We surveyed a total of 388 households in the districts of Karatu (n = 128 in five villages), Monduli (n = 114 in five villages), and Babati (n = 146 in four villages). In each village, we sampled between 17 and 40 households (Table 2). Overall, gender of respondents was relatively evenly distributed (Table 2). Proportionally, respondents in Karatu district were primarily of the Iraqw ethnicity (0.88), and were typically small-scale farmers. In Babati, and particularly in Monduli districts, a substantial share of the interviewees were Maasai (0.59 and 0.37, respectively) and lived a mainly pastoralist lifestyle (Table 2).

Table 2 Socio-demographic characteristics of interviewees in the three surveyed districts of northern Tanzania

Primary education was most common in each district, followed by secondary (and above) education but a substantial fraction of the interviewees (0.10–0.20 of respondents) had not received any formal education. In terms of dog and livestock ownership, interviewees in the three districts appeared similar, but owing to the predominant pastoralists’ ethnicities in Monduli and Babati, interviewees in these two districts usually had larger livestock herds compared to people residing in Karatu district (Table 2).

Knowledge about zoonotic diseases

Knowledge regarding anthrax, brucellosis, and rabies varied across districts (Fig. 2). In Karatu, respondents were most informed about rabies, followed by brucellosis, and least informed about anthrax. Interviewees in the district of Monduli also had greater knowledge of brucellosis, and rabies compared to anthrax. Respondents in Babati had greater knowledge of rabies, and anthrax than other districts, but comparatively little knowledge on brucellosis (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Proportional knowledge of zoonotic diseases among local residents in the districts of Karatu, Monduli and Babati in northern Tanzania. The midline represents the median, and the upper and lower limits of the box denote third and first quartile, respectively. Whiskers indicate the range and open circles represent outlier

According to generalized linear mixed models and subsequent model selection, age [based on relative variable importance (RVI) and confidence intervals non-overlapping with zero] was the major influential factor affecting anthrax knowledge in all three districts (Table 3). Regarding anthrax, the odds of scoring more points increased by 2.02–2.61 times per year of life (Table 3; please note that age was centered; i.e. the average age was scaled to zero). Gender was found to influence anthrax knowledge in both Karatu and Babati districts. In males the odds for scoring more knowledge points were 1.89–2.28 greater than for females. In Karatu, Iraqw respondents had less knowledge of anthrax than other ethnicities residing in the area. Formal education was positively associated with knowledge about anthrax, but confidence intervals of the regression coefficients overlapped with zero which implies that this relationship was not consistent or very strong (Table 3).

Table 3 Model averaged regression coefficients (β) incl. Associated 95%-confidence intervals (upper; lower), odds ratio (OR) and relative importance (RVI) of variables related to knowledge of anthrax, brucellosis and rabies in three districts of northern Tanzania

Knowledge pertaining to brucellosis was positively associated with age of respondents in all three districts. For the districts Monduli and Babati, ethnicity was also found to be an important determinant of knowledge, with members of other ethnicities knowing relatively more about rabies compared to interviewees of Maasai (Monduli) or Maasai and Arusha (Babati) ethnic identity (Table 3).

In Karatu district, gender explained some differences in knowledge about rabies. In male respondents, the odds for scoring greater knowledge points were 1.76 times greater compared to female interviewees. In this district, age positively influenced respondent’s knowledge as well. Among respondents in Monduli, interviewees with a primary education or secondary education had about two times greater odds to score more knowledge points compared to interviewees without formal education. In the Babati district, none of the regression estimates were strongly associated with knowledge of rabies (Table 3).

Practices and risk factors related to zoonotic diseases

Overall, all demographic groups within a household reportedly took care of livestock and dogs (Fig. 3) but the main demographic group differed across districts. In Karatu, adult females were often mainly responsible for livestock (0.28 of respondents), whereas in Monduli (0.06) and Babati (0.05), adult females were rarely solely handling livestock. Children were also reported (adults and children combined: 0.31–0.44) to handle livestock in all districts. Similarly, dog handling was reportedly carried out by all demographic groups, including children (Fig. 3); children were major dog handler (children and adults & children combined) in a fifth to a third (0.21–0.36) of all households. Dog ownership in Monduli and Babati was also more prevalent compared to Karatu (Fig. 3; Table 2).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Reported proportion of demographic groups (ad. = adult) responsible for livestock and dog handling among interviewed households in three districts (Karatu, Monduli, and Babati) of northern Tanzania. Scores denote the exact proportion of each bar category

The majority of interviewees in all three districts reported that milk (Karatu: 0.99; Monduli 0.77; Babati: 0.91) and meat (Karatu: 0.98; Monduli 0.88; Babati:0.94) were always boiled or cooked before consumption (Fig. 4). However, particularly in the districts of Monduli and Babati, a notable proportion of respondents reported consuming raw milk (Karatu: 0.01; Monduli 0.23; Babati: 0.09) and raw meat (Karatu: 0.01; Monduli 0.12; Babati: 0.06).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Reported preparation of milk and meat before consumption as reported by households in three districts (Karatu, Monduli, and Babati) of northern Tanzania. Scores denote the exact proportion of each bar category

Attitudes towards zoonotic diseases

Interviewees in Monduli and Babati districts frequently ranked rabies as the most dangerous disease, whereas interviewees in Karatu district rated brucellosis (closely followed by rabies) as a major threat to human and livestock health. Anthrax was consistently rated as least dangerous disease in all three districts (Fig. 5). Within all three districts, rankings of the three diseases were significantly different (Kruskal Wallis Χ2 > 25; df = 2; p < 0.001 for comparisons within districts). In all three districts, risk ranks were significantly (all p ≤ 0.001) correlated with the proportional knowledge of each disease (Karatu: tau = − 0.38, n = 384; Monduli: tau = − 0.36, n = 342; Babati: tau = − 0.39, n = 438). Given our risk scale (1 = most dangerous; 3 = least dangerous), people with greater knowledge about a specific disease tended to perceive it as more dangerous.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Local peoples’ risk perceptions of anthrax, brucellosis, and rabies in three districts (Karatu, Monduli, Babati) of northern Tanzania. For each district, we plotted the proportion of the disease rank by dividing the frequency of occurrence of each rank by the total number of respondents (Karatu n = 128, Monduli n = 114, Babati n = 146). Rank 1 displays highest risk perception, rank 3 lowest risk perceptions

Attitudes towards wildlife in relation to zoonotic diseases

In all three districts, a large share of local people (Karatu: 0.47; Monduli: 0.43; Babati 0.72) expressed that wildlife had an overall negative influence on human and livestock health (Fig. 6). However, large proportions of the interviewees, particularly in Karatu and Monduli, mentioned that wildlife had a neutral (Karatu: 0.40; Monduli: 0.40; Babati: 0.24) or even a positive effect (Karatu: 0.13; Monduli: 0.17; Babati: 0.04) on human and livestock health.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Proportion of local respondents’ attitudes (neutral, positive, negative) about the overall effect of wildlife on livestock and human health in three districts (Karatu, Monduli, and Babati) of northern Tanzania.

Discussion

Our KAP survey across three districts of northern Tanzania revealed that (1) relative knowledge about causes, symptoms, initial treatments and prevention methods regarding three zoonotic diseases (anthrax, brucellosis, and rabies) was very variable. Interestingly, (2) knowledge about specific diseases varied across districts, and (3) socio-demographic correlates of knowledge were rather variable in direction and strength with the exception of the frequently observed positive association between interviewee’s age and knowledge regarding knowledge of anthrax (all three districts), brucellosis (all three districts), and rabies (Karatu district). In addition, our survey revealed that (4) practices such as drinking raw milk and eating raw meat are practiced among a sizeable portion (reported proportional raw consumption in the three districts ranged from 0.01–0.23 for milk and 0.01 to 0.12 for meat) of the surveyed population, particularly in the districts with a greater share of ethnicities practicing pastoralist lifestyles. Finally, we show that (5) risk perceptions of diseases were positively and relatively consistently correlated with knowledge regarding specific diseases in each district.

Knowledge and practices related to zoonotic diseases

Limited level of knowledge regarding zoonotic diseases, both among the rural population [10] and among medical practitioners [9], and limited access to treatments or post exposure prophylaxis [47], constitute favorable conditions for zoonotic pathogen transmission. Beyond appropriate public health laws, education, and individual behavior can possibly minimize the transmission of infectious diseases. Identifying knowledge gaps in human populations can thus be helpful [48, 49]. KAP studies are useful for identifying such knowledge gaps across landscapes and different demographic groups of the public, yet are associated with some level of uncertainty and potential bias. Our comparative approach required standardization in the interview process and the consistent scoring of answers according to pre-defined criteria. The trade-off for this standardization may be that we have underestimated actual knowledge due to possible language barriers (e.g. Swahili may not be the first language for all interviewees) and little efforts by interviewers to prompt more answers.

Despite these possible limitations, interviewees knew comparatively little about anthrax, with slightly greater knowledge scores achieved in the districts of Monduli and Babati (Table 3). Greater knowledge in Monduli and Babati can possibly be explained by historic anthrax outbreaks in Lake Manyara National Park (which is located in Monduli district, and borders Babati district) during the 1970s and 1980s [22, 50]. Importantly, Monduli district is still considered a high risk area for anthrax, with multiple instances of mainly cutaneous anthrax cases diagnosed in health facilities in this district [23]. During the dry season of 2018 an anthrax outbreak occurred in Babati district, mainly at the eastern shore of Lake Manyara, with numerous verified anthrax cases in wildebeest Connochaetes taurinus and other wildlife species (C. Laizer, pers. comm). In Monduli, and possibly also in Babati, the consumption of bushmeat is relatively common across large fractions of local residents [51], which may be a risk factor for exposure to anthrax given the high prevalence of B. anthracis in samples of wildlife species that are usually consumed by humans [23]. Indeed, anthrax infection risk seems to be particularly high in demographic groups that are frequently involved in handling animals (milking, slaughtering, skinning) [52].

Similar to other studies [10, 53], the findings of age being a key determinant of knowledge and spatially heterogeneous distribution of knowledge, provides some circumstantial evidence that knowledge about specific diseases is possibly affected by experience of past disease outbreaks and interventions in a given area. However, this hypothesis is difficult to test given the scarcity of reliable and comparative data on actual disease prevalence in space and time. Alternatively, the effect of age may merely indicate greater accumulated knowledge over a person’s lifespan (e.g. greater likelihood of exposure to public health information campaigns). Regardless of the underlying reasons for age being positively correlated with increased knowledge, this relationship supports the need for increased health education covering zoonoses among younger generations.

Seemingly, greater potential exposure to a zoonotic disease did not necessarily equate to greater knowledge in other cases. For example, the primarily pastoralist ethnicities in Monduli and Babati knew less about brucellosis compared to other non-primarily pastoralist ethnicities (Table 3). However, these relationships may also have been blurred by actual differences in brucellosis prevalence and possibly also by language barriers.

Formal education only had limited effects on knowledge regarding zoonotic diseases. An exception may be Monduli district, where education was positively associated with knowledge for anthrax and rabies. However, given the correlative nature of our study, it is not possible to accredit greater knowledge to specific education regarding zoonotic diseases in schools per se. To our knowledge, neither primary nor O-levels school syllabi in Tanzania explicitly cover aspects of the three diseases. Similarly, we are not aware of specific information campaigns on these diseases in our study area. Yet, before employing information campaigns in the school system or outside of formal education systems, it may be instrumental to formally assess the effectiveness of these education programs.

Because other hypothesized predictors were inconsistently related to knowledge regarding the three diseases and knowledge was occasionally lower in high risk groups compared to people possibly not as exposed, we suggest that a proactive educating system in the frame of a holistic One Health approach should be implemented in our study area [52]. Such education should particularly target individuals with elevated exposure to zoonotic diseases including children who often handle livestock and dogs in our study area and are thus exposed to potential infection. To some extent, variable exposure to specific pathogens reflects differences in cultural practices, and ethnic-specific gender roles [19, 52], which can be used to better identify specific risk groups. In light of frequent interactions between children, livestock and dogs (and thus potential exposure of children to associated zoonotic pathogens), it may be worthwhile to consider including basic aspects of zoonotic risk prevention in primary school education.

Attitudes towards zoonotic diseases and risk perception

Unfortunately, there is only limited information (actual exposure risk and associated morbidity) about the relative importance of each disease in terms of public health in the region which prevents us to ascertain an objective disease risk ranking. In addition, the question was asked in a general sense which could have caused variable interpretation of the question among interviewees. Yet, considering a near 100% fatality rate in humans (if no post-exposure prophylaxis is administered), rabies may objectively be the most dangerous of the considered diseases [28, 35]. Indeed, a great proportion of respondents in Monduli and Babati rated rabies as the most dangerous disease (Fig. 5). Rabies prevalence is often underestimated in northern Tanzania [28, 54] and incidences may substantially be underreported. Apart from small-scale vaccination trials in Babati district [55], we are not aware of large scale dog (and cat) vaccination projects in the three studied districts. In light of the considerable effectiveness of mass vaccinations of domestic dogs and cats in reducing rabies [25, 27], this option should be jointly considered by public health authorities, local communities and international organizations [56] .

Attitudes towards wildlife in relation to zoonotic diseases

Our interview data (Fig. 6) largely reflect overall negative perceptions regarding wildlife in this study area [57] and suggest that – with regard to human and veterinary health – the majority of rural people mainly perceive costs associated with wildlife, and few people consider wildlife species as beneficial for human and veterinarian health. Beyond the potential for pathogen transmission, large wildlife species cause direct and indirect costs to rural population in northern Tanzania [57]. Therefore, beyond improved education on zoonotic diseases, integrated “One Health” efforts need to strengthen the veterinary and medical infrastructure (hospitals and veterinary offices; reasonable transport options to health facilities; appropriate diagnosis tools and methods), services (e.g. preventive vaccination programs; affordable or free consultation at hospitals and veterinary district offices), and effective co-operation across the human health, veterinary, and wildlife sector. In turn, such investments in public health may additionally improve wildlife conservation efforts since improvements in veterinary health services could be associated with reductions in the severity of human-wildlife conflicts, because healthier livestock are possibly less prone to attacks by large carnivores [58].

Conclusion

This KAP survey covering three diseases and spanning three districts highlights substantial knowledge gaps among the rural population in northern Tanzania. Proactively educating rural populations (i.e. before the occurrence of disease outbreaks), particularly targeting ethnic and demographic groups with elevated exposure risk to specific pathogens, could be a valuable tool to minimize transmission of zoonotic pathogens. Although clear evidence that education effectively reduces infection risk is often lacking [59], we hypothesize that education could be cost-effective method to reduce infection risk.