Background

Health researchers and providers increasingly recognize the importance of obtaining information about adolescents' perspectives of their quality of life (QOL) [110]. Several instruments have been developed for the measurement of adolescents' QOL to examine the impact of health care interventions, supportive services, and health promotion initiatives [e.g., [3, 8, 11, 12]]. These instruments typically consist of subscales that represent experiences with various conditions in life (a.k.a. life domains) that are of general relevance to adolescents, including their perceived: (a) self (e.g., self-esteem), (b) relationships with friends and family, (c) experiences at school, and (d) living environment [13, 14]. Often, the subscale scores are combined to obtain an overall, or global, QOL score. Other instruments include one or more general questions for the measurement of adolescents' global QOL in terms of their happiness or satisfaction with their lives. Despite the increasing availability of such instruments, the relationships among adolescents' self-reported health status (a.k.a. perceived or self-rated health status), their experiences with particular conditions in life, and their global QOL have not been examined extensively.

Several conceptual models have been developed to describe the relationships between health and QOL in adult s [1522]. Most of these models emphasize assessing QOL from the perspective of the individual, and are based on the general proposition that alterations in health status affect other conditions in life (life domains), such as physical and psychological functioning, and social and environmental conditions, that are relevant to a person's QOL [e.g., [15, 2022]]. For example, Wilson and Cleary [15] introduced a very useful model of health and QOL wherein alterations in one's physiological condition (e.g., disease) result in physical and psychological changes that affect functional status, general health perceptions, and global or overall QOL. Concepts pertaining to characteristics of the individual (e.g., motivation and values) and characteristics of the environment (e.g., social support) are also taken into account. However, the relationship between self-reported health status and QOL is not expounded in the model; in particular, it is not clear how self-reported health status relates to other life domains relevant to QOL.

There is compelling empirical support for the associations between self-reported health status and QOL in general adult populations. A meta-analysis by Smith, Avis, and Assmann [23] showed that variation in QOL is explained by several life domains that are affected by differences in physiological health status (e.g., the presence of disease) and symptom severity. Their "model of the determinants of quality of life" (p. 448) is based on the proposition that the life domains mediate the associations between symptom severity and physiological health status, and QOL. Their regression analyses revealed that, relative to physical and social function, mental health status was by far the most important variable explaining QOL. Beckie and Hayduk [24], using structural equation modeling, similarly demonstrated that indicators of health status could be viewed as explanatory variables of QOL. Based on a study of adults who underwent coronary artery bypass graft surgery, they found that the eight health indicators measured by the Short-Form 36-item instrument (SF-36) [25] explained 67% of the variance in QOL, and that the effects of general health perceptions and mental health status were the most substantial. They concluded that "quality of life can be considered as a global personal assessment of a single dimension, which may be causally responsive to a variety of other distinct dimensions including dimensions such as health" (p. 281).

Several other researchers have examined the associations among self-reported health status, various life domains, and global QOL in adult populations [e.g., [2628]]. However, information about these associations in adolescent populations is relatively sparse. The potential relevance of self-reported health status with respect to adolescents' QOL was shown in a study by Zullig et al. [29] who found that, in a sample of high-school students in South Carolina (U.S.A.), adolescents' self-reported health status was modestly correlated (r ranging from .09 to .22) with five life domains (satisfaction with family, friends, school, living environment, and self) and overall life satisfaction (r = .21). Other research has shown that adolescents' self-reported health status is associated with various health indicators, including physical activity, nutritional status, health-risk behavior, and physical disability [2932]. Although these studies provide support for the measurement of adolescents' self-reported general health status, the differentiation of adolescents' self-reported physical and mental health status has not been extensively examined. Consequently, it is not known to what extent adolescents differentiate their physical and mental health status and whether this differentiation is relevant with respect to their global QOL and particular life domains.

Study objectives

We designed a study to: (a) validate adolescents' differentiation of their self-reported physical and mental health status and (b) examine the associations of these variables with global QOL and several relevant life domains, including adolescents' satisfaction with their family, friends, living environment, school, and self. With respect to the first objective, we hypothesized that, relative to one another, self-reported physical health status would be more strongly associated with physical activity, and self-reported mental health status with depressive symptoms. Drawing on the previously mentioned conceptual models and empirical research on health and QOL, we further sought to obtain information about (a) the relative importance of self-reported physical and mental health status with respect to adolescents' global QOL and several life domains and (b) the extent to which the relationships among self-reported physical and mental health statusand global QOL are mediated by the life domains (see Figure 1). Global QOL is viewed here as a unidimensional construct that pertains to individuals' satisfaction with, or appreciation of, their lives overall[18, 3032]. The life domains represent adolescents' satisfaction with various conditions in life that have the potential to contribute to their global QOL [33].

Figure 1
figure 1

Structural model of the relationships between self-reported physical and mental health status, domains of life satisfaction, and global QOL. Notes: N = 6,932, WLSMV χ2 (178) = 2,083.22 - 2,010.02, RMSEA = .049, CFI = .951. The variances of all latent factors were fixed at 1.0 for model identification. The measurement structures of the latent factors for each of the life domains are identical to those reported by Sawatzky et al. [37] (these are not shown here because of space limitations). All parameter values are standardized. The corresponding unstandardized parameters are provided in Table 4. 1Self-reported physical and mental health status were modeled as two ordinal variables with a latent factor that accounts for their correlation (not shown here). *p < .05.

Methods

Sampling

The data were obtained via the British Columbia Youth Survey on Smoking and Health 2 (BCYSOSHII), a cross-sectional health survey that was conducted in 2004 to obtain information about tobacco dependence, drug and health-related behavior, and quality of life in adolescents in grades 7 to 12 in schools in the province of British Columbia (BC), Canada. The methods of this survey have been described in detail in several published studies [e.g., [3439]]. The survey avoided two regional districts within the province that were known to have very low smoking prevalence rates so as to be cost-efficient in assembling a sample of adolescents that used tobacco (the primary purpose of the principal study). Nineteen of the 60 school districts in BC were sampled to achieve maximal geographic coverage of regional districts (remote and sparsely populated areas were not surveyed). Fourteen of the school district administrators provided permission for their schools to participate. This resulted in a sample of 89 eligible schools, ofwhich 49 (42 secondary schools, 5 alternative schools, and 2 middle schools) agreed to participate. Passive parental consent was obtained by providing parents with letters that informed them of the survey. Ethical approval was granted by the Behavioural Research Ethics Board of the University of British Columbia.

The survey questionnaire was administered by research assistants during class-time hours in pen and paper format (79.6%) or through a computer-based format (20.4%). The format was primarily determined by the availability of computers in the various schools. Less than 1% of the students refused to participate and the response rate within schools was 84%, on average (non-response was mostly due to student absenteeism) [34, 36]. The resulting sample consisted of 8,225 adolescents (smokers and non-smokers).

Measurement

Self-reported physical and mental health status were measured using two questions, "How would you rate your physical health?" and "How would you rate your emotional or mental health?" with the following response options, which were taken from the general health status question of the SF-36 [25] and which are widely used in the national population health surveys of many countries: "excellent" (coded as "5"), "very good," "good," "fair," or "poor" (coded as "1"). The validity of measuring adolescents' self-reported general health status in this manner is supported by observed associations with various other health status indicators, including physical activity, nutrition, health-risk behavior, and physical disability [4043]. Study findings have consistently revealed that a relative increase in adolescents' self-reported general health status is associated with less health-risk behavior and fewer days of limited activity [41, 43].

To validate adolescents' differentiation of their physical and mental health status, we examined the relative importance of these variables with respect to depressive symptoms and the frequency of physical activities. Depressive symptoms were measured using 12 items from the Center of Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [44]. The adolescents were asked: "How often have you felt or behaved in the following manner in the past week (7 days)?" (e.g., "hopeful about the future," "happy," "lonely," "sad"). The CES-D provides four response options ranging from "rarely or none of the time (less than one day)" (coded as "0") to "most or all of the time (5-7 days)" (coded as "3"). The total score, with a possible range of 0 (no depressive symptoms) to 36, was used in the analysis. The estimated reliability of the 12 items is .87 in this sample (based on the ordinal Cronbach alpha reliability estimate [45]). Physical activity was measured using the following question adapted from several large surveys (e.g., The USA Youth Risk Behavior Survey [46] and The Ontario Drug Use Survey [47]): "On how many of the last 7 days did you exercise or participate in sports activities for at least 20 minutes that made you sweat and breathe hard? If none, enter '0' days. Please include activities such as basketball, jogging, swimming, cross-country skiing, hockey, or dance, that you participated in either at school or outside of school."

An abridged version of Huebner's Multidimensional Students' Life Satisfaction Scale (MSLSS) [48] was used to measure adolescents' satisfaction with five life domains, including their family (4 items), school (4 items), living environment (2 items), friends (4 items) and self (4 items) [37]. The original MSLSS consists of 40 items, of which 10 are negatively worded. The psychometric analyses reported by Sawatzky et al. [37] revealed that the adolescents may not have interpreted and responded to all items in the same way. There were inconsistencies in the responses to the negatively worded items and several other items. An abridged 18-item version was developed by identifying those items that were found to be most invariant (all positively worded). Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) provided support for its construct validity when allowing for a few theoretically defensible modifications [37]. The same measurement structure was used to represent the five life domains as latent factors in the study reported herein. The ordinal Cronbach alpha reliability estimates [45] of the abridged subscales with four items were ≥ .80 in this sample. A six-point ordinal response format (with response options ranging from "strongly disagree" (coded as "1") to "strongly agree" (coded as "6")) was used [49].

Global QOL was measured with two items. The adolescents were asked to appraise their QOL using a picture of an eight-rung ladder (Cantril's self-anchoring ladder [50], referred to here as the QOL-ladder) (see Figure 2). The bottom run was coded as "1" and the top as "8". The adolescents also were asked to rate their agreement with the statement, "I am satisfied with my quality of life" with four response options ranging from "strongly disagree" (coded as "1") to "strongly agree" (coded as "4"). General questions of this nature, including Cantril's self-anchoring ladder, have been widely used in surveys for the measurement of various concepts such as global QOL [5153]. A latent factor explaining the variance in both of these variables was used to represent global QOL.

Figure 2
figure 2

Quality of life ladder. Notes: Derived from Cantril's self-anchoring ladder [50]. An error resulted in 8 rungs being presented in the paper-based version whereas 10 rungs were presented in the computer version. To remedy this, we rescaled the QOL-ladder for the computer- and paper-based versions to their common denominator by multiplying the computer-based version of the QOL-ladder by 0.8 and rounding the resulting scores to zero decimals.

The adolescents were asked to indicate their age and sex, and to answer several questions about their ethnic identity and living arrangements. Ethnic identity was determined by asking, "How would you describe yourself?" The 12 response options were adapted from Statistics Canada's [54] classification of "visible minorities" (e.g., "white/Caucasian," Aboriginal/First Nation, Chinese, South East Asian). The adolescents selected one or more responses, which were subsequently grouped as: "white/Caucasian," Asian (including Chinese, Japanese, Korean, South East Asian, and Filipino), Aboriginal/First Nation, and "other." With respect to their living arrangements, the adolescents were asked, "Which parent or parents do you currently live with most of the time?" with eight response options (i.e., mother, father, step-mother, step-father, guardian(s), foster parent(s), grandparent(s), and other please specify).

Statistical methods

Structural equation modeling was used to examine the hypothesized relationships by fitting a latent variable mediation model to the sample data (see Figure 1). The variances of the latent factors were specified to equal one to avoid indeterminancy and to set the metric of the latent factors [55]. Polychoric correlations were used to avoid obtaining biased parameter estimates due to the ordinal distributions of the observed variables [5658]. The MPlus 5.2 software [59] was used to estimate the model parameters by specifying a probit link function and using a mean and variance adjusted weighted-least squares estimation method (WLSMV) suitable for ordinal data [60]. Model fit was evaluated with several global fit indices, and by comparing the differences between the implied and the observed polychoric correlation matrices. Adequate model fit was defined by a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of < .06 [61] and a comparative fit index (CFI) of ≥ .95 [62]. In addition, the pattern and magnitudes of the residual correlations were examined to locate any specific areas of misfit [63, 64]. The percentage of residual correlations with absolute values greater than .10 is provided as a summary of this direct comparison.

The relative importance of the explanatory variables was determined by the Pratt index (d) [65], which quantifies each variable's contribution to the explained variance (irrespective of the magnitude of the R-squared), measured as a percentage. The extent to which the two relationships between global QOL and physical and mental health status were mediated by the life domains was evaluated as the division of the indirect-effects (mediated by the life domains) and the total effect (the sum of the direct- and indirect-effects for the associations between global QOL and physical and mental health status), expressed as a percentage [66]. The standard error of the indirect effects was calculated using the Delta method, which is similar to the approach used in the Sobel test [67].

Of the 8,225 adolescents, 920 did not provide responses to any of the MSLSS questions. The analysis was limited to those who responded to the global QOL items, the items measuring mental or physical health status, and at least one of the MSLSS items (N = 6,932). Multiple imputation (MI) [68] was used to impute any remaining missing responses (2.5% imputed data). The results were compared with those obtained using MI for the subsample of adolescents who provided a value for at least one of the analysis variables (N = 8,174; 13.9% imputed data). The SAS 9.2 software package [69] was used to create 10 imputed datasets for the MI analyses, following the guidelines offered by Allison [70] and Enders [71], to assess convergence and to incorporate auxiliary variables (i.e., demographic variables (sex, ethnicity, school grade), symptoms of depression, and two variables pertaining to the adolescents' experiences at school).

Results

Sample description

The sample consisted of an approximately equal proportion of boys and girls in grades 7 through 12 (see Table 1). The average age was 15.2 years (SD = 1.5, n = 8,054) with 7,964 adolescents being between 12 and 18 years. Although most of the adolescents who identified their ethnicity (n = 7,882) self-identified as "white/Caucasian" (72.6%), the sample also included Aboriginal adolescents (16.5%), Asian adolescents (Chinese, Japanese, Korean, Filipino, or South-East Asian) (5.8%), and adolescents belonging to one or more other groups (5.1%). A sizeable percentage (17.3% of 7,994 adolescents) indicated regularly speaking a language other than English, and 6.9% of 8,058 reported being born in a country other than Canada.

Table 1 Sample description

Most of the adolescents agreed or strongly agreed to being satisfied with their QOL (82.3% of 7,606 adolescents) (see Table 1). The mode of the QOL-ladder responses was at level 6 of 8 rungs (36.7%), with 11.9% of the adolescents reporting the best possible life, and 14.0% providing a rating at or below the middle of the scale (≤ 4) (n = 7,675).

The measurement of self-reported physical and mental health status

The joint- and marginal-distributions of self-reported physical and mental health status are provided in Table 2. The corresponding conditional distributions provide support for adolescents' ability to differentiate these variables. For example, 9.5% of the adolescents who rated their physical health as good or better rated their mental health as fair or poor, and 5.3% of the adolescents who rated their mental health as good or better rated their physical health as fair or poor. The polychoric correlation was .55, indicating a shared variance of only 30% among these two (underlying) variables. With respect to the differentiation of mental and physical health status (see Table 3), we found that 94% (d, Pratt Index) of the explained variance in depressive symptoms (R2 = 35.5%) could be attributed to mental health status (the remaining 6% was attributed to physical health status). Conversely, relative to self-reported physical health status, self-reported mental health status accounted for only 18% of the explained variance in physical activity (R2 = 7.7%) (see Table 3).

Table 2 Joint and marginal distributions of self-reported physical and mental health status
Table 3 Relationships between self-reported physical and mental health status and depressive symptoms and frequency of physical activity

The associations between health status and quality of life

The hypothesized model with the life domains operating as mediators of the relationships between self-reported physical and mental health status and global QOL resulted in acceptable overall fit (WLSMV χ2 ranging from 2,083.22 to 2,010.02 for the 10 MI datasets (N = 6,932), RMSEA = .049, CFI = .951, residual correlations ranging from -.07 to .07) (see Figure 1). Satisfaction with family, friends, school, living-environment, and self, and self-reported physical and mental health status explained 76.1% of the variance in global QOL. Although self-reported physical and mental health status were bivariately significantly correlated with global QOL (r = .49 and .70, respectively), their associations were substantially smaller, albeit statistically significant, in the multivariate model (see Table 4). The life domains also were bivariately significantly correlated with global QOL. However, relatively small and statistically non-significant regression coefficients were obtained for satisfaction with friends and satisfaction with school in the multivariate model (see Table 4). These variables accounted for less than 2% (d, Pratt Index) of the explained variance relative to the other variables in the model (see Table 4). Global QOL was mostly explained by satisfaction with self (d = 42%), self-reported mental health status (d = 30%), and satisfaction with family (d = 20%). Self-reported physical health status accounted for only 3% of the explained variance.

Table 4 Relative importance of variables explaining global QOL

Self-reported physical and mental health status were significantly correlated with each of the life domains (r physical health ranging from .22 to .45; r mental health ranging from .27 to .54), and they predominantly explained satisfaction with self (R2 = 33.0%), and, to a lesser extent, satisfaction with family (R2 = 16.9%), friends (R2 = 11.3%), and living environment (R2 = 14.2%) (see Table 5). Only 7.9% of the variance in satisfaction with school was explained by self-reported physical and mental health status. Relative to self-reported physical health status, most of the variance in each of the life satisfaction dimensions could be attributed to the adolescents' self-reported mental health status (d ranging from 68% to 87% for each of the life domains) (see Table 5).

Table 5 Relative importance of variables explaining the dimensions of life satisfaction

The parameters for the relationships between physical and mental health status, the life domains, and global QOL were used to determine the magnitude of the total and the indirect relationships between physical and mental health status and global QOL as mediated by each of the life domains (see Table 6). The standardized total effect on global QOL was larger for self-reported mental health status (β = .61), while adjusting for self-reported physical health status, than for self-reported physical health status (β = .17), while adjusting for self-reported mental health status. These relationships were partially mediated by the life domains (67.8% total mediation for physical health and 45.4% total mediation for mental health status). The relationships between the two health status variables and global QOL were primarily mediated by satisfaction with self (54.0% mediation for self-reported physical health and 29.1% mediation for self-reported mental health) and, to a lesser extent, by satisfaction with family (10.8% mediation for self-reported physical health and 13.7% mediation for self-reported mental health).

Table 6 Mediation effects for physical and mental health status and global QOL

Discussion

This study provides support for (a) the notion that adolescents can differentiate between physical and mental health when they provide reports of their health status and (b) the relevance of this differentiation with respect to five life domains and global QOL. The results revealed that relatively poorer self-reported physical and mental health status were significantly associated with lower global QOL and lower satisfaction with each of the life domains. The adolescents' global QOL was predominantly explained by mental health status and by their satisfaction with self and family. Satisfaction with self and family were the main mediating variables for the relationships between mental health status (45.4% total mediation) and physical health status (67.8% total mediation) and global QOL.

Other studies have shown that self-reported general health status is significantly associated with health-promoting and health-risk behavior [4043] and with various life domains and global QOL [29]. Our study contributes to this area of research by providing preliminary support for the validity and the relevance of distinguishing between adolescents' self-reports of their physical and mental health status. The findings suggest that, relative to one another, self-reported mental health status is more strongly associated with depressive symptoms and physical health status with physical activity. Although further research is needed to examine the validity and relevance of these variables with respect to other research objectives (e.g., their associations with particular health-risk behavior), the current findings suggest that the use of two self-report items for the measurement of adolescents' physical and mental health status could contribute valuable information in population-based adolescent health surveys.

There were substantial differences in the associations between self-reported physical and mental health status and adolescents' global QOL and the five life domains. The correlations with self-reported mental health status were greater than were those with physical health status. This finding is congruent with a study by Zullig et al. [29] who found that, relative to the self-reported number of days with poor physical health, the number of poor mental health days was more strongly correlated with adolescents' overall life satisfaction (r = -.27 versus -.15) and their satisfaction with their family (r = -.25 versus -.14), friends (r = -.10 versus -.07), living environment (r = -.15 versus -.10), school (r = -.15 versus -.12) and their self perception (r = -.29 versus -.21). However, in our study, the correlations with global QOL (r physical health = .49; r mental health = .70), and each of the life domains (r physical health ranging from .22 to .45; r mental health ranging from .27 to .54;) were relatively stronger. It is possible that the measurement of self-reported physical and mental health status (rather than the number of poor physical and mental health days), and the use of the abridged MSLSS for the five life domains (rather than the use of single items for each of the life domains), resulted in greater sensitivity to detect these associations.

In addition to these bivariate associations, our study provides information about the relative importance of self-reported physical and mental health status and the five life domains in explaining global QOL in adolescents. The results revealed that self-reported physical health status contributed minimally to global QOL when controlling for the other variables in the model; its association with global QOL was significantly confounded by self-reported mental health status and the five life domains. Self-reported mental health status was relatively more important with respect to each of the life domains, and it was the second most important explanatory variable for global QOL. These findings provide support for attending to the mental health needs of adolescents.

With respect to each of the life domains, we found that most of the variance in global QOL could be attributed to the adolescents' satisfaction with themselves and their families. The associations between satisfaction with friends and school and global QOL were not statistically significant in the multivariate model. These findings are congruent with a study by Gilman [72] who found that, in a sample of 321 high-school students in a Southeastern US state, the associations between satisfaction with friends and school and global QOL were relatively small when controlling for the other life domains. It is possible that adolescents' satisfaction with their friends and their school is associated with their satisfaction with their family, and that these associations are therefore confounded in the multivariate model. This is an important area for further study.

An important theoretical conclusion to be drawn from these findings is that self-reported physical and mental health status and the life domains can be viewed as conditions that contribute to global QOL in adolescents. These relationships are fundamentally different from those implied by the common practice of deriving global QOL scores from the combined scores of particular life domains. Many multidimensional instruments designed to measure QOL are based on the assumption that scores pertaining to various life domains can be combined so as to obtain an overall (general) QOL score. For instance, it has been argued that an overall QOL score could be obtained by averaging the scores of the five life domain subscales of the MSLSS [49, 73, 74]. The theoretical premise of this approach is that the experiences in the various life domains reflect, or arise from, a common source, labeled global QOL. This premise is not congruent with the previously noted conceptualization of life domains as conditions that contribute to QOL. Our analyses demonstrate a different approach that is congruent with the conceptualization of QOL as a global concept that is partially explained by various contributing conditions, such as health status and people's experiences with various other aspects of life (life domains) [23, 24, 2628, 32, 33].

There are several limitations to this study that must be taken into account. First, the cross-sectional nature of this analysis does not warrant conclusive statements about the causal nature of the relationships. Claims pertaining to the direction and causal nature of these relationships require further investigation. Second, although care was taken to limit the bias that may have resulted from missing data, it is possible that there were systematic differences between the adolescents who did not respond to all the items in comparison with those who did. Third, it is possible that different magnitudes of the observed relationships would be obtained in different populations, or groups, of adolescents. For instance, the relative importance of the life domains may be different for boys and girls or for adolescents from different age-groups or cultural or socio-economic backgrounds. We therefore recommend further research to examine the differences in the magnitudes of the associations between health status, important life domains, and global QOL in different adolescent populations.

Conclusions

This study provides support for a conceptual model of self-reported physical and mental health status and several life domains that contribute to adolescents' global QOL. Support is also provided for the use of distinct items to measure self-reported physical and mental health status in adolescent population health surveys. Mental health status and, to a lesser extent, physical health status were associated with significant differences in the adolescents' appraisals of their family, friends, living environment, school experiences, self, and their global QOL. Questions pertaining to these important life domains require more attention in health assessments and in population health research so as to target appropriate supportive services for adolescents with mental or physical health challenges.