Background

Worldwide, there is an increasing interest in the measurement of quality of life (QoL) outcomes, which are useful in the assessment of physical, mental and social well-being and are potentially useful for informing healthcare decision making [1, 2]. However, QoL assessments within Asian contexts have been limited due to the lack of questionnaires with demonstrated validity and reliability among Asians and the problem is particularly acute in the Asian pediatric population.

Although several children QoL questionnaires have been developed [3], very few were culturally adapted and validated for use in Asia. At the same of this study (2003), we identified only three questionnaires (in Arabic, Korean, Hebrew and Turkish) that were validated in Asian pediatric populations – Child Health Questionnaire, Childhood Health Assessment Questionnaire, and Quality of Life in Childhood Cancer [48]. However, only one of these questionnaires is a generic (Child Health Questionnaire) QoL questionnaire while the others are disease-specific questionnaires which have limited applications for the general pediatric population.

Furthermore, these language versions were not used in most countries in Asia, in particular Southeast Asia. Hence, we undertook a study to evaluate the psychometric properties of KINDL® – a generic children's quality of life questionnaire among English-speaking Singaporeans [9]. English was chosen because it is a universal language with potentially wide application in several Asian countries, including Hong Kong, the Philippines and Malaysia.

It was encouraging that since our first study was published [1], several more QoL questionnaires became available for use in the Asian pediatric population [1013]. A few other QoL validation studies among Asians [1417] were also published at around the same time as our study. However, only two of these studies evaluated factor validity of the questionnaire used [13, 18]. In addition, language availability remains a problem. In the case of Singapore, as many as 11% (44,756 of 412,796) of Singaporeans aged 15–24 understood English only [19]. Hence, an English version of a generic QoL questionnaire is needed if such studies are to be representative of the general pediatric population.

In cross-cultural adaptation and translation of QoL questionnaires, it is important to ensure that psychometric properties of the original questionnaire are preserved in the new cultural setting. In our earlier study among children aged 8–16 years [9], internal consistencies of KINDL-Kid was 0.75 and that of KINDL-Kiddo was 0.84. The general accepted Cronbach's alpha for group level comparison was >= 0.5 [20, 21] and that for person level comparison was >= 0.8 [21]. Hence, our results showed that the KINDL questionnaires were satisfactory for group level comparison and less satisfactory for person level comparison. This might suggest the translated KINDL questionnaire might be psychometrically different from the source English version. We aimed to confirm this hypothesis using a large school-based sample (who are likely to be representative of the general pediatric population). Thus, the aim of this paper is to evaluate the psychometric properties, in particular, factor structure of the Singapore English version of KINDL questionnaires among school-going children.

Methods

Study design and subject recruitment

This study was conducted over several days in March 2002. Permission to conduct the study in school children was obtained from the Ministry of Education, Singapore. Invitations for participation were sent via either facsimile or e-mail to primary and secondary schools (equivalent to the junior and high schools in the United States). A follow-up facsimile or e-mail was sent a week after the first mailer stating the study aims and design. Further details were provided to those principals who agreed to participate in the study.

Students aged 8–16 years old were selected by convenience sampling by the teacher in-charge of the survey. The aim was to minimize administrative burden so as to encourage participation. As English is the primary medium of teaching in Singapore, subjects completed the Singapore English versions of KINDL questionnaires and a simple demographic information sheet by self-completion without any assistance.

Instrument

The Singapore English versions of KINDL questionnaires were cross-culturally adapted [9] from the source Germany English versions [22] (available with permission from the developers), using standard guidelines, including independent forward and back translation and cognitive debriefing in focus groups. KINDL questionnaire is available in three age versions: KINDL-Kiddy (4–7 years), KINDL-Kid (8–12 years) and KINDL-Kiddo (13–16 years) with both parents and self-reports. In this study, we used the self-report versions of KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo. Each KINDL questionnaire comprised 24 items (with five answer categories) yielding a general QoL score (TOTAL) and six subscales: physical health (PH), general health (GH), family functioning (FAM), self-esteem (PER), social functioning (FREN) and school functioning (SCH) scores (Table 1). Reverse scoring was applied to some items so that higher item scores represent better QoL. Item scores were summed up to give subscale scores, and subscale scores summed up to give a TOTAL score. The raw score was transformed to a scale of 0–100 to facilitate interpretation.

Table 1 Items in KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo

Statistical analyses

The number of responses at minimum (floor) and maximum (ceiling) scores were reported. Internal consistencies of individual subscales were evaluated using Cronbach's alpha. Floor and ceiling effects exceeding 15% have been considered high [23]. In our study, however, we applied a more stringent criteria with floor and ceiling effects exceeding 5% considered as high. Factor structure of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) were evaluated using principal component analysis with varimax rotation. The criterion chosen to determine that an extracted factor accounted for a reasonably large proportion of the total variance was based on an eigenvalue greater than 1. There is no consensus on the minimum sample size for factor analysis, with recommendations ranging from 100 to 300 [24, 25]. We hypothesised that KINDL (Singapore) will retain the same factor structure (Table 1) as the source KINDL (Germany). All statistical analyses were performed using STATA [26].

Results

Subjects

A total of 181 e-mail and faxes was sent with follow-up e-mail or faxes. In response, three primary (one girl's school, two co-ed) and five secondary schools (one boy's school, one girl's, three co-ed) agreed to participate. The most common reason that principals gave to decline the invitation was that their students were already involved in other studies. A total of 328 respondents (mean (standard deviation, SD) age: 9.6 (1.31) years; 67% female; 75% Chinese, 16% Malays, 9% Indians and others) completed KINDL-Kid (Singapore) while 1,026 respondents (mean (SD) age: 14.0 (1.00) years; 82% female; 82% Chinese, 12% Malays, 6% Indians and others) completed KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore). There were no missing data.

Data analyses

Mean, Standard deviation (SD), Floor and Ceiling Effects (Table 2)

Table 2 Score distributions, floor and ceiling responses and internal consistency of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) subscales

Mean (SD) of the TOTAL QoL score was 65.5 (12.76) and 56.6 (11.92) for KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore), respectively. For KINDL-Kid (Singapore), floor effect was negligible for all subscales except PER, where number of response at minimum scores was 7.3%. Ceiling effect was more pronounced with all subscales except PER having ceiling responses exceeding 5%. For KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore), similar to KINDL-Kid (Singapore), floor effect was negligible for all subscales except PER, where floor response was 5.6%. However, ceiling effect was less pronounced than in KINDL-Kid (Singapore), with only one (FAM) of six subscales having ceiling responses that exceeded 5%.

Internal consistency (Table 2)

KINDL-Kid (Singapore): Internal consistency as measured by Cronbach's alpha was 0.79 for KINDL TOTAL and ranged from 0.40 (SCH) to 0.71 (FAM) for the subscales. None of the scales had internal consistencies that were considered acceptable for person level comparisons. Overall these internal consistencies were lower than the alphas reported for the original German version (alpha = 0.63 to 0.84), which however were calculated across both KINDL-Kid and KINDL-Kiddo [27].

KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore): The overall internal consistency of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) was higher than KINDL-Kid (Singapore). Cronbach's alpha was 0.83 for KINDL TOTAL with Cronbach's alpha ranging from 0.44 (SCH) to 0.84 (PER) for the subscales. Internal consistencies of the TOTAL and the subscales PER and FAM were considered acceptable for person level comparisons. Some internal consistencies were lower than the alphas reported for the original German version (alpha = 0.63 to 0.84) [27], while others were higher.

Table 3 Factor structure of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) questionnaires

Factor analysis (Table 3)

KINDL-Kid (Singapore): KINDL-Kid (Singapore) items did not load onto the six factors originally hypothesised. Instead, eight factors were generated. The cumulative percentage of total variance explained by the eight-factor solution was 60.4%. A six-factor solution would have explained 51.3% of the total variance. One of the new factors comprised of GEN4, FAM4 and SCH4, items that were originally from three different subscales (Factor 2, Table 4). In addition, some items were reorganized and loaded onto subscales different from those originally hypothesised. For example, GEN1 loaded onto FREN instead of GEN (Factor 5, Table 4).

Table 4 Distribution of items based on factor structure of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) questionnaires

KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore): Results of factor analysis was more reassuring for KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) compared with KINDL-Kid (Singapore). Seven factors were identified (eigenvalue > 1.0). The cumulative percentage of total variance explained by the seven-factor solution was 62.2%. A six-factor solution would have explained 57.8% of the total variance. Factors 1, 3 and 4 (Table 4) of KINDL-Kid (Singapore), corresponded exactly to PH, PER and FAM subscales of KINDL-Kiddo (Germany). Factors 2 and 5 of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) were almost identical to GEN and FREN subscales of KINDL-Kiddo (Germany) except for an exchange of two items GEN4 and FREN4 between the two subscales. Interestingly, Factors 6 and 7 actually comprised of items (two per factor) that were from the SCH subscale of KINDL-Kid (Germany).

Discussion

In this study, we evaluated the psychometric properties of KINDL (Singapore) questionnaires among English-speaking Singaporean children and adolescents recruited from schools. The psychometric properties of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) in this Asian population were reassuring albeit with a few areas needing improvement. First, internal consistency was good for the TOTAL instrument score. Two subscales displayed problems with regards to floor and ceiling effects. Internal consistency was sometimes insufficient in the subscales, but apart from this was good for group level comparisons. However, we should aim to achieve internal consistency good enough for person level comparisons. Yet, this is technically difficult as respondents were relatively young, and their responses are likely to be less reliable than adults. Arguably the homogeneity of the sample – comprising of healthy children and adolescents only with presumably fewer variation in their QoL trait parameter values – had led to slightly reduced Cronbach alpha values. Second, factor structure of KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) was almost identical to KINDL-Kiddo (Germany), except for the partitioning of SCH subscale into two factors in KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore). This could be addressed by modifying one or more items in SCH subscale.

The psychometric properties of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) were less reassuring and needs to be improved in several aspects. Otherwise, cross-cultural comparisons of findings made using KINDL-Kid questionnaires could be misleading because the equivalence of the construct measured is questionable. First, ceiling effects needs to be reduced, possibly by including items of greater difficulty. Second, internal consistency needs to be improved by either eliminating items that do not contribute to a subscale or by developing new items with better psychometric properties and/or modifying existing items. Third, we would suggest to further examine the items in KINDL-Kid (Singapore) either by modifying existing items or adding new items such that the factor structure reflects that of the KINDL-Kid (Germany). This is because in a focus group study among Singaporean children and adolescents [27], we found that Singaporean and Western children/adolescents share a remarkably similar notion of general and health-related QoL. Hence, it is theoretically possible for both KINDL-Kid (Singapore) and KINDL-Kid (Germany) to achieve the same factor structure.

We recognized there are limitations in this study. First, our sample is unlikely to be representative of the general pediatric population given the very low participation rate (4.5%) from the school community. Second, at the school level, we opted for convenience sampling in order to improve participation rate. As a result, there was an over-representation of female subjects (general population: 50.5% female [28]) and slight under-representation of the ethnic minorities (general population: 13.9% Malays and 9.3% Indians and others [28]). Nevertheless, the results were encouraging and provided empirical support for further studies to evaluate KINDL (Singapore) questionnaires. Third, Singapore is a multi-ethnic, multi-lingual society but we have used only the English language questionnaires. However, as English is the main language (besides mother tongue) used in all educational institutions, this is unlikely to pose a problem. It should be noted that there were no subjects who had to be excluded from the study because they could not understand English.

Conclusion

KINDL-Kiddo (Singapore) exhibited good psychometric properties and may be used to assess quality of life in this multi-ethnic English-speaking Asian population. However, the psychometric properties of KINDL-Kid (Singapore) need to be improved. Furthermore, to cater to the multi-lingual sociocultural environment in Singapore and other Asian countries, various language versions will be needed. For the moment, it is strongly recommended to focus on KINDL TOTAL score when interpreting KINDL data.