Anesthetic agents are powerful tools for the scientific investigation of consciousness.1,2 The state transitions that occur at the induction of and emergence from general anesthesia provide a controlled environment for studying the neural correlates of consciousness (NCC).3 As their name implies, NCCs need to be correlated with conscious phenomena. Although many aspects of conscious experience are inherently subjective and not accessible to an external observer, the ability to respond to verbal commands is widely considered the gold standard for assessing the level of consciousness in the context of general anesthesia. A preserved ability to respond to verbal commands indicates consciousness of the external world (i.e., connected consciousness)4 and is abolished by adequate general anesthesia.5

Table Summary of patient and control characteristics

Assessing the ability to respond to verbal commands is commonly used in research protocols.6,7,8,9,10,11,12 but it carries significant limitations. First, it allows only intermittent assessments of the level of consciousness. Second, it offers low time resolution with intervals of 10–15 seconds between commands. Third, it provides only a categorical measure (correct or absent response) and may also yield equivocal responses. Fourth, stimuli (e.g., sound intensity and background noise level, sex of the speaker, tone and intonation, live vs recorded commands, free-field stimulus delivery vs earphones) are not standardized across studies. Fifth, the onset of anterograde amnesia is difficult to assess because of the repetitive nature of the stimuli (e.g., “make a fist” repeated many times). Sixth, patient responses to the commands often cause head and face movements that introduce artefacts in electroencephalographic (EEG) or functional imaging recordings. Lastly, it is hard to rule out the possibility, though remote, that anesthetic agents may impair the language processing required to understand and respond to the commands before inducing a state of disconnected consciousness.4

The primary goal of this study was to evaluate the combination of the “object hold” technique13 and handgrip dynamometry (i.e., a measure of the force applied during handgrip holding) for providing a continuous measure of volitional control over the forearm and hand muscles during anesthesia induction in surgical patients. We also assessed recall of events during anesthesia induction.

Methods

This study was approved by the Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital’s Research Ethics Board (6 May, 2014) and its report follows the STROBE initiative reporting guidelines.14

Patients

After obtaining informed consent, we tested 15 American Society of Anesthesiologists physical status I or II patients, aged 18–65 yr and scheduled for elective spinal surgery. Fourteen age-matched healthy non-anesthetized volunteers were recruited as controls. Exclusion criteria included a body mass index greater than 32 kg·m−2, history of drug abuse, obstructive sleep apnea, symptomatic gastroesophageal reflux, significant cardiovascular, respiratory, neurological, or psychiatric disease, and any other medical condition that would render a slow induction of anesthesia inappropriate.

Dynamometry

Each patient’s maximal grip strength was measured on the morning of surgery (and before entering the operating room) using a handgrip dynamometer (Vernier Software and Technology, Beaverton, OR, USA) with Vernier Logger Lite software15 according to the National Institute of Health motor strength protocol assessment.16 The patients were instructed to hold the dynamometer in a vertical position (Fig. 1) with their dominant hand using as much force as necessary to reliably prevent its fall, but not more. They then practiced the task until we confirmed a satisfactory performance. Force data were acquired at a sampling rate of 100 Hz using Logger Lite software. After a submaximal practice attempt, the patients were asked to squeeze the object in their hand as strongly as possible. Two trials one minute apart were obtained with the higher force recorded. Maximal grip strength was also measured for healthy volunteers. Those subjects with a maximal grip strength two standard deviations or more below the mean for their age and sex were excluded.17

Fig. 1
figure 1

Dynamometer. The left panel shows the dynamometer with the metal disc attached at the inferior end to obtain a total weight of 1.2 kg. The right panel shows the holding position

For the subsequent intraoperative handgrip dynamometry assessments, pilot data had shown that a weight of approximately 1.2 kg was suitable for the object drop technique. That is, it was light enough to be easily held but heavy enough to fall quickly when the gripping force ceased. In addition, a lighter object may remain in the hand despite complete absence of grip force because of the natural resting flexion of the fingers. Since the original dynamometer from the manufacturer was lighter, we modified it by attaching a 0.9 kg metal disc at the bottom to achieve a total weight of 1.2 kg (Fig. 1).

Study protocol

After arriving in the operating room, electrocardiogram and pulse oximetry monitors were applied. A blood pressure cuff was placed on the patient’s non-dominant arm. A plastic arm board and cushions were used to support the patient’s dominant forearm. A venous cannula was then placed on the non-dominant arm. No processed EEG monitoring was used because of the expected temporal delay in detecting a transition from the awake to the anesthetized state.18 The patient was then instructed to close their eyes, relax, and focus on holding the dynamometer in a vertical position for as long as possible. A facemask was used to provide 100% oxygen. Lidocaine (40 mg) was administered intravenously to mitigate any discomfort associated with a propofol infusion. The automated blood pressure measurements were placed on hold. Sixty seconds of dynamometer data were acquired before starting the propofol infusion. An Infusomat® Space intravenous infusion pump (B Braun Melsungen AG, Melsungen, Germany) was used to infuse propofol (Fresenius Kabi Canada Ltd, Toronto, ON, Canada) at a rate of 1 mg·kg−1·min−1 (based on the actual body weight) and was stopped when the dynamometer fell from the patient’s hand. No other verbal instructions were given to the patient during induction, but the patient’s ability to respond to commands was assessed immediately after the object fall by loudly and repeatedly asking the patient to open their eyes. In doing so, we sought to determine whether the patient remained capable of responding to verbal commands immediately after the object fall to compare the relative sensitivity of the object hold and response with verbal commands.

The automated blood pressure measurements were then resumed, and the attending anesthesiologist completed the induction sequence with sufentanil and additional propofol to attenuate the hemodynamic response to tracheal intubation. Rocuronium was used to facilitate endotracheal intubation.

The object hold time was defined as the interval between the start of the propofol infusion and the time when the object fell from the patient’s hand. We chose the object hold protocol because it does not require periodic stimulation or instructions during induction and because it has excellent temporal resolution. Integrating force sensors in the object (i.e., using a dynamometer) made it possible to continuously measure the force exerted to hold the object during induction, until the patient ultimately dropped the object. The object hold time was the same as the infusion duration because the infusion was stopped immediately after the object fall.

The control subjects were instructed to perform the same task with the same object—i.e., they held the weighted dynamometer in a vertical position with their eyes closed until told to stop. Control subjects received no anesthetics and were not coached during the task. Five minutes of dynamometric data were acquired for each control subject to quantify the changes in grip strength that occurred over time without anesthesia.

We also tested three additional patients (two men) who completed the same propofol infusion protocol, except that every five seconds they were asked to squeeze (80% maximal force) the dynamometer. This was done to rule out the possibility that the progressive decrease in holding force observed during induction (described in the Results section) reflects muscular weakness rather than the central effects of propofol.

Postoperatively, the patients completed a modified Brice19 questionnaire to assess their memories of the induction protocol and to screen for intraoperative awareness.

Statistical analysis

In the absence of previously published data on grip force during induction of anesthesia, we chose a convenience sample size of 12–15 patients with an equivalent number of non-anesthetized controls. The study was conducted from January 21, 2018 to April 26, 2018, inclusively. Matlab (Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA) was used for all analyses unless stated otherwise. The Kaplan-Meier estimate20 was used to calculate the empiric cumulative distribution function of the object hold task, using the Matlab script ecdf.m. Handgrip strength was normalized for every subject by dividing the force values by the average force recorded during the first 60 seconds for that specific subject. Mean force curves were then generated for the patient and control groups and down-sampled to 1 Hz by computing the average force for every second. Separate analyses were conducted after alignment of the data based on the time of infusion start or of object drop. Means between the two groups were then compared at one-second intervals using sequential t tests. To obtain an exact specification of the experiment-wise error rate despite the large number of t tests, we used a method based on permutations.21 Because the number of possible permutations was very large, we used a bootstrap routine to obtain a subset of 10,000 samples of all possible permutations. We used the Matlab script data sample without replacement. This method yields the t test threshold required to obtain the desired experiment-wise error rate, which we set at P < 0.05. The critical t value based on bootstrap resampling was 3.47 and 3.45 for the propofol and fall alignment, respectively (corresponding to P ≈ 0.001 uncorrected for 26 df).

A linear mixed-model22 was used to analyse grip force changes in the 50 seconds preceding object drop as a linear function of time assuming that the patients share a common slope but allowing for different intercepts. This approach allows for repeated measures. We modelled the outcome yij as:

$${\text{y}}_{\text{ij}} = \, \alpha_{\text{i}} + \, \beta_{0} + \, \beta_{ 1} {\text{x}}_{\text{j}} + \, \varepsilon_{\text{ij}}$$

where:

  • 1- yij is the grip force of patient i (i = 1,2,…,14) at time j

  • 2- αi is the difference between the random intercept of patient i and β0

  • 3- β0 is the regression constant (intercept)

  • 4- β1 is the regression coefficient (slope)

  • 5- εij is the error term, accounting for unexplained errors (e.g., measurement error).

The model, which contains both fixed (the influence of time) and random (baseline value of each patient) effects, was estimated with SAS PROC MIXED using an unstructured covariance structure (SAS software version 9.3, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).22 The goodness of fit was assessed for each site separately with the concordance correlation coefficient (CCC) that ranges from -1 (perfect discordance) to 1 (perfect concordance), with zero indicating no relationship.23,24 To compute the CCC, we used the SAS macro written by Xianming Tan (then at the Biostatistics Core Facilities, McGill University Health Centre Research Institute) for our previous publication.25

Simple linear regression with Pearson’s R coefficient was used to model the decrease of force (using the average force) as a function of time for the data aligned on the time of infusion start. This could not be done with multiple regression data because the number of patients still holding the dynamometer decreased with time (see Fig. 2). Simple linear regression with Pearson’s R coefficient was also used to examine the effects of demographics on object hold time. For both mixed-model analysis and simple regression, the assumptions were met and inspection of the residuals revealed no anomaly. We used Jeffreys’ method26 implemented by SAS PROC FREQ to calculate the two-sided confidence interval for the proportion of patients who did not respond to verbal commands after the object drop (i.e., the proportion of patients considered unconscious by both the object hold and response to verbal commands methods). Two samples t tests were used to compare the characteristics of patients and controls.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Handgrip holding force as a function of time (mean ± standard error) for patients (red) and controls (blue). Time zero corresponds to the start of the propofol infusion for patients. The control data were adjusted to have the same object holding time as the patients. The proportion of patients still holding the dynamometer is shown by the Kaplan-Meier curve (black step graph). The data are shown from 30 seconds before the infusion start to 124 seconds after infusion start. This limit was chosen because only two patients held the object for longer than 124 seconds after infusion start. The standard error for patients widens towards the end of the recording because the number of patients decreases. The green line represents patient data points that significantly diverged from control data using a t threshold of 3.47 (corresponding to an experiment-wise P < 0.05) The dashed black line shows the regression fit for data that exceed the t threshold

Results

Fifteen patients and 14 controls completed the study. The data from one patient were excluded because of marked agitation and erratic movements of the dominant arm during induction. The characteristics of the patients and controls are summarized in the Table.

The evolution of handgrip force as a function of time after the start of the propofol infusion is shown in Fig. 2 for controls and patients. A progressive decrease in grip force became apparent very soon after starting the infusion. The decrement slopes became more pronounced approximately 50 seconds after the start of the infusion. The difference in grip force between controls and patients was non-significant (P > 0.05; Blair permutation test) from -30 to 73 seconds after the start of the infusion; it was significant (P < 0.05; Blair permutation test) from 74 seconds to the end, and from that point onwards there was a linear decrease of force as a function of time. Simple linear regression based on the average force revealed a very good linear fit (dashed line; R2 = 0.98; P < 0.001).

Figure 3 shows the same data after alignment to the time when the object fell, which was set to zero. A progressive decrease in grip force became apparent approximately 100 seconds before the object fell and the rate of decrement became more pronounced approximately 70 seconds before the object fall. The difference in grip force between controls and patients was non-significant (P > 0.05; Blair permutation test) until 52 seconds before the object fall. The difference was significant (P < 0.05; Blair permutation test) for the interval from 51 seconds before the fall to the fall time. From 51 seconds before the fall onwards, there was linear decrease of force as a function of time. Mixed-model analysis revealed a significant linear decrease of grip force as a function of time with a very good fit, as indicated by a CCC (the equivalent of R2) of 0.84 (P < 0.001; SAS PROC MIXED) and by inspection of the residuals (Supplemental Figure; available as Electronic Supplementary Material).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Same as Fig. 2 except that time zero corresponds to the object fall and that the number of patients is constant.14 The green line represents patient data points that significantly diverged from control data using a t threshold of 3.45 (corresponding to an experiment-wise P < 0.05). The dashed black line shows the regression fit for data that exceed the t threshold

Figure 4 shows the data from one of the three additional subjects who were asked every five seconds to squeeze the dynamometer. There was a progressive decrease in the handgrip force applied to hold the object despite a preserved ability to exert stronger force when asked to. This shows that progressive decrease in holding force is not caused by muscle weakness.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Dynamometry of a patient that was asked every five seconds to squeeze the object. The upper panel shows the full data range; the bottom panel provides an enlarged view of the data in the grey rectangle above to better show the progressive decrease of the holding force between the squeezes. Time zero corresponds to the start of the propofol infusion

Patient age (24–65 yr) negatively correlated with object drop time (R2 = 0.47, P < 0.01) (Fig. 5). Weight, NIH maximal grip strength, and mean grip force during the 60 seconds baseline period showed no significant relationship with object drop time.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Linear regression of object drop time as a function of patient age

Of the 17 patients who completed the object hold task (14 with the standard protocol and three with intermittent squeeze requests), 16 (94%; 95% confidence interval [CI], 76 to 99%) did not respond to verbal commands after dropping the dynamometer. One patient in the standard protocol opened his eyes on command immediately after the object fall but failed to do so when the command was repeated ten seconds later. During the postoperative interview, no patient reported any memories of dropping the object or hearing any verbal commands. There were no cases of confirmed intraoperative awareness with recall.

Discussion

Herein, we have described a method that continuously assesses volitional control during the induction of general anesthesia. Dynamometry offers several advantages over conventional measures of consciousness for studying the events that occur during induction of anesthesia. First, dynamometry offers a sustained, uninterrupted measure, available at every time point during induction. Therefore, recordings such as the electroencephalogram or electromyogram, can be related to this behavioural measure at all times instead of at intervals only. Second, the time when the object is dropped can be determined with greater precision than the time when the ability to respond to verbal commands ceases. Third, dynamometry yields a continuous (used here to mean that the number of possible values is only limited by the accuracy of the measuring instrument) variable (i.e., the force exerted), which provides more information than the binary categorical variable obtained by assessing response to verbal commands.

Dynamometry likely revealed changes in the patients’ mental state before they dropped the object as revealed by the inflection point about 80 seconds before the object fall (Fig. 3) and by the subsequent progressive reduction in holding force. Combining dynamometry with neurophysiological measures could help elucidate anesthetic mechanisms in ways that are not currently possible using categorical measures of conscious states.

Another advantage of the object hold technique is that it does not require any instructions or other actions by the patient during induction. This reduces the risk of movement artefacts on the electroencephalogram or during functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Heavily sedated patients often move their head or open their eyes when responding to verbal commands, even if the commands do not call for ocular or head movements. The object hold technique also facilitates standardization across studies (as long as the object weight and positioning are similar). It is worth noting that traditional measures of consciousness can be combined with dynamometry. For example, patients could be periodically asked to open their eyes, or squeeze the dynamometer, as we have done with three patients.

Our findings can be compared and contrasted with the work by Tsai et al.27 who used both handgrip and bite force measurements during sedation and found that propofol administered by target-controlled infusion increased bite force in a concentration-dependent manner during minimal and moderate sedation (predicted effect-site concentrations: 0.4–1.6 µg·mL−1). These authors also found that propofol increased handgrip force, but only at the 0.8 µg·mL−1 plateau. Both bite and handgrip force later decreased at the 2.0 µg·mL−1 plateau. There is no clear explanation for their variable findings but additional data from EEG or functional MRI might offer some explanation.

Concordance with response to verbal commands

There was good concordance (agreement rate [95% CI] between the two methods was 94% [76 to 99%]) between the estimation of loss of consciousness with the object hold technique and response to verbal commands since all but one patient failed to respond to verbal commands given immediately after the fall of the object. Nevertheless, a larger sample size will be required to definitely assess the level of concordance between the two methods of assessment. That said, the generally good concordance that we observed is consistent with the report of Cummings et al.13 who concluded that “the time measured when the patient dropped a water-filled syringe … approximated most closely to their clinical impression of the ‘true’ induction time”. Future studies may seek to describe the effect of object weight and patient factors on the sensitivity and specificity of this technique when compared with other measures of consciousness such as response to verbal commands.

Neurophysiology of handgrip and propofol

Single-cell recording experiments performed with non-human primates have revealed linear relationships between the firing rate of pyramidal tract neurons (PTN) in the primary motor cortex and the force output of various muscle groups.28,29,30,31 Furthermore, MRI studies have shown that blood-oxygen-level dependent signals increase linearly and expand over a greater cortical surface area when human subjects increase handgrip force.32,33,34 Thus, it appears that handgrip force is directly related to both the number and discharge rates of recruited PTNs. Our finding that handgrip force decreases linearly during induction of anesthesia suggests that propofol progressively reduces the number and/or discharge rate of the PTNs involved with the handgrip task. Impairment of neuronal communication by propofol1 can in principle account for the reduced number of active PTNs. We thus propose that handgrip dynamometry provides a continuous behavioural measure of impaired neuronal communication caused by general anesthetics.1 Studies combining handgrip dynamometry with electroencephalographic recordings or functional MRI are needed to validate this proposal.

It is generally agreed that the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)A receptor is the principal target of propofol in the mammalian brain, where it enhances chloride currents and inhibitory post-synaptic potentials.35,36 Since GABAA receptors are present on PTNs,37 direct inhibition of these neurons may contribute to the effects of propofol on handgrip.

Although handgrip force progressively decreased during the induction of anesthesia, patients retained their capacity to consciously exert significantly greater forces (as shown by the three additional patients who responded to verbal commands for increased handgrip) throughout the induction period, presumably through the recruitment of additional PTNs (Fig. 4). This finding may be related to propofol-induced impairment of neuronal communication.1 Additional research is needed to explain why the force exerted decreases during induction despite a preserved ability to exert a much stronger force.

Limitations

There are limitations to this study. First, we are not proposing that dynamometry be introduced into clinical practice. We developed this method for research protocols that would benefit from a continuous measure of the patient’s behaviour. Second, the object hold paradigm cannot be used during the maintenance or emergence phases of anesthesia. Third, we used a single, relatively slow propofol infusion rate (1 mg·kg−1·min−1) and a single object weight (1.2 kg). Thus the effect of propofol infusion rate and object weight has not been examined. Fourth, although our approach could probably be adapted for use in studies using target-controlled infusion, we have not attempted to do so. Future studies may also consider using lean body weight to calculate infusion rates, especially in obese patients.38 Fifth, the critical inflection points in the Fig. 2 and Fig. 3 data undoubtedly precede the time when the difference between patients and controls reach statistical significance. Sophisticated methods for time-series analysis, such as change-point analysis39 or hidden Markov chain models40 could potentially help identify these critical points and could provide valuable insights. Sixth, there is no direct evidence that the neural events responsible for the progressive decrease in handgrip holding force are related to the neural events that ultimately cause unconsciousness. They may simply be independent events, both caused by propofol. The study of the changes in handgrip force during induction of anesthesia will nevertheless help us understand the effects of general anesthetics on the human brain.

Conclusion

The most important finding of this study is that the handgrip force exerted to hold an object decreases progressively during induction of anesthesia with propofol and that this force decreases despite a preserved ability to exert a much stronger force. Further studies could aid in the detailed understanding of this observation and may ultimately help explain how anesthetics impair consciousness.