Skip to main content
Log in

The Relation between Emotion and Intellect: Which Governs Which?

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article contrasts two beliefs about the relation between the emotions and the intellect. Each sees emotional responses as fundamental and primary sources of thinking. Each sees a different role for the intellect following emotional responses to worldly phenomena. L. S. Vygotsky, articulating a belief common at his time, sees the intellect as a disciplining force, one that, after a pause or interlude, serves to temper emotions to produce a “catharsis” and what he calls “intelligent emotions,” those subjected to rational thought and a higher plane of cognition than either emotion or intellect could produce alone. Jonathan Haidt, following Vygotsky by nearly a century, asserts that emotions control cognition, rather than as Vygotsky conceives, being subordinated by reason. Haidt, in the tradition of David Hume and with more empirical data than Vygotsky provides for his view, sees the passions ruling human thought and action. Any accompanying reason serves to rationalize gut feelings rather than to control them; reason, Haidt argues, is a “rationalist delusion” that gives emotional thinking the veneer of reason. This article outlines both positions and attempts to reach a synthesis of their views.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I retain all language from sources without endorsing it when it includes a sexist orientation.

  2. People who have co-existed with elephants are not necessarily like Haidt’s hapless rider struggling to control the uncontrollable (Mackenzie & Locke, n. d.). Thai people, for instance, have served for many generations as elephant keepers, working more relationally with them. This phenomenon is on display in the films of martial arts star Tony Jaa, who grew up in rural Thailand alongside wild elephants and incorporates them into his movies. A stunning example appears in “The Lord of the Elephants,” a scene from Ong Bak 2 in which he controls not just an elephant, but a herd of them (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=uQG8y0kJqdY). Of course, this scene was orchestrated and edited, but in interviews the filmmakers have talked about the difficulties of filming with elephants, and how those who grew up with them have developed relationships that enable them to direct and act with them in movie scenes.

References

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., Engeström, Y., & Vasquez, O. (1997). Mind, culture, and activity: Seminal papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1983). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haenen, J. P. P. (1993). Piotr Galperin: His lifelong quest for the content of psychology. Doctoral dissertation, Proefschrift Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, NL.

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Vintage.

  • Hareli, S., Kafetsios, K., & Hess, U. (2015). A cross-cultural study on emotion expression and the learning of social norms. Frontiers in Psychology. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01501/fullhttps://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01501

  • John-Steiner, V. (1987). Notebooks of the mind: Explorations of thinking. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive developmental approach. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior: Theory, research, and social issues (pp. 34–35). Holt Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontiev, A. N. (1997). On Vygotsky’s creative development. In L. S. Vygotsky, Collected works (Vol. 3: Problems of the theory and history of psychology) (pp. 9-32) (R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock, Eds.; R. van der Veer, Trans.). Plenum.

  • Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations (M. Cole, Ed.; M. Lopez-Morillas & L. Solotaroff, Trans.). Harvard University Press.

  • Lim, N. (2016). Cultural differences in emotion: Differences in emotional arousal level between the East and the West. Integrative Medicine Research, 5, 105–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackenzie, K., & Locke, P. (n. d.). Of humans and elephants. International Society of Ethnobiology. Retrieved from http://www.ethnobiology.net/humans-elephants/

  • Roth, W.-M. (2007). Emotion at work: A contribution to third-generation cultural-historical activity theory. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 14(1–2), 40–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(6), 2408–2412. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908239106

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smagorinsky, P. (2001). If meaning is constructed, what is it made from? Toward a cultural theory of reading. Review of Educational Research, 71, 133–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smagorinsky, P. (2011). Vygotsky’s stage theory: The psychology of art and the actor under the direction of perezhivanie. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 18, 319–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smagorinsky, P. (2018). Emotion, reason, and argument: Teaching persuasive writing in tense times. English Journal, 107(5), 98–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Veer, R. (1997). Translator’s foreword and acknowledgements. In L. S. Vygotsky, Collected works (Vol. 3: Problems of the theory and history of psychology) (pp. v-vi) (R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock, Eds.; R. van der Veer, Trans.). Plenum.

  • Van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1971/1925). The psychology of art (Scripta Technica, Inc., Trans.). M.I.T. Press. Partially available at http://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1925/index.htm

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987a/1934). Thinking and speech. In L. S. Vygotsky, Collected works (Vol. 1, pp. 39–285) (R. Rieber & A. Carton, Eds.; N. Minick, Trans.). Plenum.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987b). Lecture 4. Emotions and their development in childhood. In L. S. Vygotsky, Collected works (Vol. 1, pp. 325–337) (R. Rieber & A. Carton, Eds.; N. Minick, Trans.). Plenum.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1999). The teaching about emotions. Historical-psychological studies. In The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 6: Scientific Legacy) (R. Rieber, Ed.; M. J. Hall, Trans.) (pp. 71–235). Plenum.

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yaroshevsky, M. (1989). Lev Vygotsky (S. Syrovatin, Trans.). Progress Publishers.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Smagorinsky.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Smagorinsky, P. The Relation between Emotion and Intellect: Which Governs Which?. Integr. psych. behav. 55, 769–778 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-021-09637-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-021-09637-5

Keywords

Navigation