Introduction

The neuromodulator adenosine is involved in many physiological and pathological CNS processes [13]. Although the actions of adenosine are reasonably well characterised [4, 5] the mechanisms of extracellular adenosine production are complex [3, 6, 7]. A large number of studies, over many years have shown that adenosine can be released from brain tissue by a number of different stimuli (including hypoxia, ischemia, depolarisation, electrical stimulation, etc.) and by a variety of mechanisms (for review, see [7]). For example, an increase in extracellular adenosine release could occur following extracellular ATP metabolism (ATP can be released by exocytosis [79] or through gap junction hemi-channels [10]). Adenosine can also be transported from the cell cytoplasm by equilibrative transporters [7, 11, 12]. Recent data has described the release of adenosine [13] which has the properties of exocytosis (Ca2+ and action-potential dependence) but there is currently no direct evidence for either adenosine exocytosis or vesicular storage. Once released, adenosine is removed by conversion to inosine (by adenosine deaminase) and taken up into neurones and glia by transporters (equilibrative or concentrative).

We have previously shown that focal electrical stimulation within the molecular layer of cerebellar slices can release adenosine [13]. This process is both Ca2+- and action-potential-dependent, and several lines of evidence suggest that parallel fibre (granule cell axons) activity is required for adenosine release [13]. To show definitively that adenosine is released from parallel fibres in cerebellar slices is difficult, as parallel fibres could activate a downstream target which then releases adenosine. Thus, we have taken the approach of investigating whether isolated granule cells (in culture) can release adenosine with a similar mechanism to that observed in slices. We have used microelectrode biosensors, which respond in real-time to changes in purine concentration [14, 15]. This is the first time that such biosensors have been used with primary cultured cells.

Methods

Preparation of granule cell cultures

The method for preparing granule cell cultures is based on [16].

Solution composition (quantities for one preparation)

Solution 1 (for 100 ml)

NaCl 124 mM, KCl 5.4 mM, NaH2PO4 1 mM, glucose 14.4 mM, phenol red 28 μM, HEPES 20 mM, MgSO4 150 μM and 0.3 g bovine serum albumin. The solution is filtered (sterile 0.2 μm filter) and stored at 4°C.

Solution 2

Ten millilitres of solution 1, DNaseI 0.1 mg/ml, trypsin inhibitor from soy bean 0.52 mg/ml (Invitrogen) and 3.2 mM MgSO4.

Solution 3

Ten millilitres of solution 1, 130 μΜ CaCl2 and 2.6 mM MgSO4.

Neurobasal medium

Twenty millilitres of Neurobasal (Fisher), glutamine 1.5 mM, penicillin 10 U/ml streptomycin 10 μg/ml and 400 μl B-27 (50×, Fisher).

 

The cerebellum was removed from a postnatal day 7–8 (P7–8) Wistar rat and transferred into Krebs solution (NaCl 124 mM, KCl 5.4 mM, NaH2PO4 1 mM, glucose 14.4 mM, phenol red 28 μM, HEPES 20 mM, MgSO4 150 μM and bovine serum albumin 3 mg/ml). Meninges and blood vessels were removed and following trituration and centrifugation (302 G, 3 min), cells were incubated for 15 min at 37°C in Krebs solution containing trypsin (0.025%). After centrifugation (302 G, 3 min), cells were resuspended in Krebs solution, containing trypsin inhibitor (0.52 mg/ml) and DNAse (0.1 mg/ml), and passed through a 40-μm cell strainer. The suspension was spun at 484 G for 5 min and cerebellar granule neurons were purified by a pre-plating step in Neurobasal medium (1 h, 37°C). For the harvesting of glia, coverslips were placed on the bottom of the dish so that the glia could stick to them. The suspended neuronal cells were plated on poly-l-ornithine coated coverslips (6–12 coverslips) at an approximate density of 400,000 cells per coverslip. Neurons were cultured in Neurobasal medium, containing 25 mM KCl, 2% B27, 1.5 mM glutamine, penicillin 10 U/ml streptomycin and 10 μg/ml. For experiments in which the proliferation of glial was reduced, the mitotic inhibitor (1 μM β-d-arabinofuranoside hydrochloride) was added to the Neurobasal media 24 h after culture preparation. The mitotic inhibitor was then present in all media.

Immunohistochemistry

Following recording, coverslips were incubated in 4% paraformaldehyde (in 0.1 M phosphate buffered saline, PBS) for 45 min at room temperature. The coverslips were then blocked in 10% horse serum 90% immunosolution (0.1 M PBS, 0.1% BSA, 0.1% sodium azide, 0.4% triton X-100 and pH 7.4) for 1 h at room temperature. Coverslips were washed three times (5-min washes) in 0.1 M PBS. The coverslips were incubated with primary antibody to MAP2 (Mouse, Sigma, M4403) and GFAP (Chick, Millipore, AB5541) at 1 in 500 for 2 h at room temperature. Coverslips were washed three times (5-min washes) in 0.1 M PBS. The coverslips were incubated with the secondary antibody (one in 500) for 1 h at room temperature. The secondary for GFAP was goat anti-chick 594 (Fisher) and for MAP2 was goat anti-mouse 488 (Fisher).

Preparation of cerebellar slices

Transverse slices of cerebellar vermis (400 µm) were prepared from male Wistar rats, at postnatal days 7–25 (P7–25). As described previously [13] and in accordance with the U.K. Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act (1986), male rats were killed by cervical dislocation and decapitated. The cerebellum was rapidly removed and slices were cut on a Microm HM 650 V microslicer in cold (2–4°C) high Mg2+, low Ca2+ aCSF, composed of (mM): 127 NaCl, 1.9 KCl, 8 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 1.2 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 d-glucose (pH 7.4 when bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2, 300 mOSM). Slices were stored in normal aCSF (1.3 mM MgCl2, 2.4 mM CaCl2) at room temperature for 1–6 h before recording.

Recording from granule cell cultures and cerebellar slices

An individual granule cell culture (on a coverslip) or a cerebellar slice was transferred to a recording chamber, submerged in aCSF and perfused at 6 ml/min (30–32°C). The coverslip/slice was placed upon a suspended grid. All solutions were vigorously bubbled (95% O2/5% CO2) and all tubing had low gas permeability (Tygon). Purine biosensors were positioned just above the surface of the culture/slice (bent so their longitudinal surface was parallel to the culture surface or parallel to the molecular layer of cerebellar slices). Cultures were electrically stimulated with a concentric bipolar electrode (FHC) placed on the coverslip amongst the cells. Cultures were washed with aCSF for 20–30 minutes (to remove culture medium) before recordings were made.

Biosensor characteristics

Biosensors were obtained from Sarissa Biomedical Ltd (Coventry UK). Biosensors consisted of enzymes entrapped within a matrix that was deposited around a Pt or Pt/Ir (90/10) wire etched to ∼50 μM [14, 15]. The biosensors had an exposed length of ∼500 μm that was coated with enzymes and thus capable of detecting purines. Biosensors were screened, which greatly reduced the responses to electro-active interferents (such as 5-HT, noradrenaline, dopamine and ascorbate). Four types of purine biosensor were used in this study. Firstly, a screened null sensor, possessing the matrix but no enzymes, was used to control for the release of any non-specific electro-active interferents. Secondly, screened biosensors containing adenosine deaminase, nucleoside phosphorylase and xanthine oxidase and (responsive to adenosine, inosine and hypoxanthine, ADO biosensor). Thirdly, screened biosensors containing nucleoside phosphorylase and xanthine oxidase (responsive to inosine, hypoxanthine, INO biosensor). And finally, screened ATP biosensors which consisted of the entrapped enzymes glycerol kinase and glycerol-3-phosphate oxidase [15]. Glycerol (2 mM) was included in the aCSF, as glycerol is a co-substrate required for ATP detection. A full description of the properties of the biosensors has already been published but they show a linear response to increasing concentration of analyte, are fast to respond and have a rise time less than 10 s [14, 15]. Biosensors were calibrated with known concentrations (10 μM) of adenosine, inosine and ATP. Adenosine biosensors had approximately equal sensitivity to adenosine, inosine and hypoxanthine. For non-differential ADO biosensor recordings, the concentration of purines was assessed from the adenosine or inosine calibration and is equivalent to µmol/L′ [17]. The screening of sensors was assessed by applying 10 μM of 5-HT. Calibration was performed before the culture/slice was present in the perfusion chamber and after the culture/slice had been removed. This allowed measurement of any reduction in sensitivity during the experiment. Biosensor signals were acquired at 1 kHz with a Micro CED (Mark 2) interface using Spike (Vs 6.1) software.

Drugs

All drugs were made up as 10–100 mM stock solutions, stored frozen and then thawed and diluted with aCSF on the day of use. Adenosine, inosine, NBTI, and dipyridamole were purchased from Sigma. Erythro-9-(2-hydroxy-3-nonyl) adenosine (EHNA) was purchased from Tocris–Cookson. TTX was purchased from Ascent Scientific. ATP was purchased from Roche. The mitotic inhibitor β-d-arabinofuranoside hydrochloride was purchased from Sigma.

All values are mean ± SEM. Statistical significance (P < 0.05) was assessed using the Student t test.

Results

Stimulation of granule cell cultures causes purine release

We have identified an action-potential and Ca2+-dependent form of adenosine release in the cerebellum [13]. One of the most likely sources of the adenosine is parallel fibres (granule cell axons). Thus, we have investigated whether isolated granule cells (in culture) can release adenosine with a similar stimulus. Initially, granule cells were cultured with and without high (25 mM) K+ concentrations in the media. Granule cells without the high K+ concentration died after a few days and thus all experiments were conducted with cells cultured in a high K+ concentration (which lasted up to ∼4 weeks).

Biosensors were placed upon the surface of individual granule cell cultures (on glass coverslips). Using cultures that were 2–3 weeks old, we were unable to detect purine release following electrical stimulation, even with prolonged high-frequency trains (no difference between ADO and null sensors, n = 10 coverslips, data not shown). This is not surprising as the stimulating electrode delivers a focal electrical stimulus, which will only activate a small number of cells directly under the stimulating electrode. In the slice, granule cell axons are aligned in tracts (within the molecular layer) and thus a large number will be activated by a focal stimulus. Thus, a stimulus was used which will depolarise all the cells in the culture, i.e. bath application of excitants. Depolarisation of the cells with 10–50 mM KCl did not produce reliable purine release (the ADO biosensor response was the same as the null sensor, n = 10 coverslips, data not shown). Application of 100 μM–1 mM glutamate also did not reliably release purines (n = 12 coverslips, in most coverslips (ten out of 12) there was no purine release, in two cultures a small amount of purine release was detected). However, the simultaneous application of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate, reliably (14 out of 16 coverslips) induced a large current (270 ± 53 pA, range 49–813 pA) on the ADO biosensor with little or no current on the null sensor (Fig. 1a). The stimulus (25 mM KCl + 1 mM glutamate) had no direct effect on the ADO biosensor, as once the biosensor was moved up from the culture surface the stimulus no longer produced a current (n = 3, Fig. 1b). Thus, the current is produced by detection of released purines and is equivalent to 1.8 ± 0.2 μM′ of purines (see “Methods” section).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Purine release from granule cell cultures. a Traces from an ADO and null sensor placed on the surface of a 2-week granule cell culture (sensors were bent so that the sensing area was parallel to the culture surface). Increasing the concentration of K+ from 3 to 25 mM with simultaneous application of 1 mM glutamate produced a large current on the ADO biosensor but no response on the null sensor. Inset, trace from a superimposed with a calibration trace (10 μM adenosine, dotted line) illustrating the speed of the current rise due to purine release is not defined by biosensor kinetics. b Trace from the same culture as a but the ADO biosensor has been lifted from the culture surface. Application of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate had no effect on the ADO biosensor baseline. c Superimposed traces from an adenosine biosensor (ADO) and an ATP biosensor placed on the surface of a 2-week culture. Although a large concentration of adenosine was released (following increase in K+ concentration from 3 to 25 mM plus 1 mM glutamate) no ATP could be detected. d Superimposed traces from an adenosine biosensor (ADO) and an ATP biosensor placed on the surface of a 2-week culture. Application of a high voltage stimulus (15 V) from a stimulating electrode placed equidistant between the ADO and ATP biosensors produced the release of ATP (by electroporation). However, there was very little breakdown of the released ATP to adenosine. e Superimposed traces from an adenosine biosensor (ADO) and an ATP biosensor placed on the surface of a 2 week culture. Application of exogenous ATP (100 μM) produced a rapid rise on the ATP biosensor and a much smaller, slower rise on the ADO biosensor. Thus, the conversion of ATP to adenosine is relatively slow and is not complete

The current produced by purine release had a slow time to peak (100–150 s) and a slow decay (500–600 s, Fig. 1a). As calibration curves for the sensors had a much faster rise and decay (∼20 s), the kinetics of the ADO biosensor current illustrates the kinetics of release from the cultures which is slow and prolonged (Fig. 1a, inset). Although most cultures (88 %) responded to a single stimulus, subsequent stimuli often produced a much smaller release of purines (45 ± 15% reduction, n = 12 coverslips) suggesting a depletion of internal purine stores. However, this was not always the case, with some cultures releasing similar amounts of purines with two to four stimuli (n = 4 coverslips). With a reduced stimulus, 10 mM KCl + 100 μM glutamate or 100 μM–1 mM glutamate alone, it was sometimes possible to release smaller amounts of purines (0.5 ± 0.7 μM′, n = 6 coverslips) which was sustainable for repeated stimuli (four to five, not illustrated). However, purine release (with this weaker stimulus) was not reliable, with many cultures (48%) not responding (but still able to release purines with the stronger stimulus, 25 mM KCl + 1 mM glutamate).

No ATP release can be detected

By replacing the null sensor with an ATP biosensor, we investigated whether the purines detected arose from the breakdown of extracellular ATP. The strong stimulus could possibly result in cell death leading to ATP liberation from the cytoplasm. In all experiments, no ATP could be detected following stimulation (n = 15 coverslips, detection limit ∼60 nM, Fig. 1c). Electroporation of cells (with a 15–25 V stimulus) released ATP (0.9 ± 0.2 μM) but only a very small amount of the released ATP was converted to adenosine (∼4%, n = 3, Fig. 1d). Addition of ATP (100 μM) to the cultures resulted in the slow formation of a small amount of adenosine (less than 2%, n = 6, Fig. 1e). Thus, although ectoATPases are present within the cultures, they do not appear to be efficient enough or in sufficient concentration to prevent ATP detection. Thus, we have no evidence that the released purines arise from the breakdown of extracellular ATP.

The purine released is not adenosine

We have used ADO biosensors (which detect adenosine, inosine and hypoxanthine) and INO biosensors (which detect only inosine and hypoxanthine) to determine which purines are released. Following a stimulus (25 mM KCl + 1 mM glutamate) the time to peak of currents on ADO and INO biosensors was very similar and so were the current amplitudes (once corrected for sensitivity, n = 5, Fig. 2a). This suggests that the analyte detected is not adenosine but is either inosine or hypoxanthine as they can be detected by both biosensors. This contrasts with purine release in response to hypoxia in the hippocampus, where the signal on the ADO biosensor has a much faster time to peak than the signal on the INO signal demonstrating the release of adenosine [17]. Application of adenosine (10 μM) to cultures produced only a small, slow rising current on the INO sensor (equivalent to 0.35 ± 0.2 μM inosine, i.e. only 3.5% of adenosine is broken down to inosine, n = 5, Fig. 2b). Thus, it is unlikely that the inosine/hypoxanthine detected by the biosensors arises from the rapid extracellular breakdown of released adenosine. To confirm this, we have used EHNA. EHNA blocks the conversion of adenosine to inosine (by inhibiting adenosine deaminase), thus prevents the detection of adenosine and inhibits conversion of adenosine to inosine by adenosine deaminase within the cultures [12]. EHNA (20 μM) had no significant effect on the currents measured by ADO biosensors (Fig. 2c, n = 5). As a control, we checked that EHNA blocks the detection of adenosine by ADO biosensors [12] (Fig. 2d, n = 3). Thus, the purine released is not adenosine but instead is either inosine or hypoxanthine.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The purines released are adenosine metabolites. a, Superimposed traces from an ADO biosensor (dotted line) and INO biosensor placed on the surface of a 2-week culture. Addition of the stimulus (25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate) caused an increase in current on both biosensors. The traces have been corrected for sensitivity to inosine and have similar rise-times and amplitudes. b Superimposed traces from an ADO and INO biosensor placed on the surface of a 2-week culture. Application of 10 μM adenosine produced a large current on the ADO biosensor and a much smaller and slower rising current on the INO biosensor. The arrow illustrates that the ADO current is large when a current on the INO trace is first observed. c Superimposed traces from an ADO biosensor in control (solid line) and in 20 μM EHNA (dotted line). The same ADO biosensor was placed on two different cultures. The stimulus (25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate) was applied in control conditions on one culture and after incubation (10–15 min) with 20 μM EHNA on the other culture. EHNA had little effect on the ADO biosensor current and thus the current must arise from the direct release of adenosine metabolites (inosine and or hypoxanthine). d Trace from an ADO biosensor. The current produced by adenosine (10 μM) was blocked by co-application of EHNA (20 μM) and recovered in wash. The dotted line is the baseline before adenosine application

The release of inosine/hypoxanthine is via equilibrative transport

We have investigated the mechanism of purines release. Unlike electrical stimulation in cerebellar slices [12], purine release was not blocked by TTX (1 μM Fig. 3a, n = 3) and thus does not require the firing of action potentials. In six experiments, removal of extracellular Ca2+ (substitution with equivalent concentration of Mg2+) prevented purine release (Fig. 3b). However, in some experiments (n = 3), release was still observed when Ca2+ was removed. By using an ATP biosensor we discovered that when purine release occurred in Ca2+ free aCSF, it was accompanied by a rapid release of large amounts of ATP (∼3 μM Fig. 3c, n = 3). In those cultures where no current was produced on the ADO biosensor there was also no ATP detected (n = 2 coverslips) and thus the adenosine detected probably arises from ATP metabolism. The mechanism of ATP release is unclear but could be via hemi-channels [10] or could represent cell death and subsequent release of cytoplasmic contents.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The mechanism of purine release. a Trace from an ADO biosensor placed on the surface of a 2–3-week-old culture. Pre-incubation in 1 μM TTX does not prevent purine release following application of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate. b Trace from an ADO biosensor placed on the surface of a 2–3-week culture. Application of Ca2+-free aCSF (Mg2+ increased to compensate for Ca2+ removal) caused a fall in the baseline current. Addition of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate did not cause the release of purines but instead caused another fall in baseline. However, washing the Ca2+ back in (whilst stimulus was still present) resulted in purine release. c Traces from ATP and ADO biosensors placed on the surface of a 2–3-week culture in zero Ca2+ aCSF. Application of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate caused the release of ATP and adenosine, inosine, or hypoxanthine. d Trace from an INO biosensor placed on the surface of a 2–3-week culture. Pre-incubation with NBTI (5 μM) and dipyridamole (10 μM) prevented purine release following application of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate. Following wash off, NBTI/dipyridamole (15–20 min) purine release occurred following the stimulus. e Graph plotting mean concentration of purine released in control and in NBTI/dipyridamole illustrating that NBTI/dipyridamole significantly reduced purine release. Graph summarises data from six experiments

Studies on cortical neuron and glia cultures have shown that inosine can be released by equilibrative transport [18]. To test if this mechanism occurs with granule cells cultures, we have investigated the effects of the ENT blockers NBTI (5 μM) and dipyridamole (10 μM) [19]. Following the application of these transport blockers (for 10–15 min) the release of purines was either greatly reduced or in some cultures abolished (0.94 ± 0.2 vs. 0.17 ± 0.05 μM′ Fig. 3d,e n = 6). Following wash, release often recovered (Fig. 3d) although the amount of recovery was variable. NBTI and dipyridamole did not produce any current on the ADO biosensor themselves and thus there appears to be very little basal uptake of purines within the cultures.

A reliable method of inducing adenosine release in cerebellar slices is to inhibit adenosine kinase (with iodotubercidin) this results in the efflux of adenosine via equilibrative transporters [20]. However, application of iodotubercidin (2–4 μM) had no effect on the baseline current of ADO biosensors (n = 4, data not shown) and thus basal adenosine kinase activity in the cultures must be low (as a control, release could be evoked by KCl and glutamate, following iodotubercidin application). Purines can also be released from cerebellar slices by hypoxia [21]. However, prolonged hypoxia (10–15 min, bubbling with N2/C02) had no effect on the ADO biosensor baseline (data not shown) and thus does not induce purine release in cultures (n = 4 coverslips). Thus, the major mechanism of purine release from the cultures is via equilibrative transport.

Purine release depends on culture composition

During the preparation of cultures we took steps to remove glial cells (see “Methods” section and [16]) and thus assumed that the purine release occurs from granule cells. To confirm this and to investigate cell viability, we used immunohistochemistry. GFAP antibodies were used to identify glial cells and MAP-2 (neuronal dendritic protein) antibodies to identify granule cells. In cultures 2–3 weeks old, cultures were mixed, with granule cells growing on a layer of glial cells (n = 6 coverslips, Fig. 4a). During the culture period, the small number of glia present during cell isolation must proliferate. Thus, the purine release measured could occur from either neurones or glia. There was no difference in the morphology or number of granule cells in stimulated (25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate) or unstimulated cultures, suggesting the stimulus does not produce wide spread cell death. To test the importance of glia in the release of purines we took two approaches: firstly, we assumed that 3–4-day-old cultures would contain very few glial cells, as there is little time for proliferation. This was confirmed by immunohistochemistry (Fig. 4b, n = 5). We were unable to record any purine release from these cultures (25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate, n = 10 coverslips). In 7-day-old cultures, purine release occurred in ∼50% of cultures (n = 6 out of ten coverslips). In such cultures, the number of glia had increased (not illustrated). By 2 weeks, purine release was very reliable (see earlier). Although the reliability of purine release matches glial cell proliferation, it is also possible that neuronal differentiation occurs over this period and changes in the expression of proteins (such as ENTs) could account for the differences in purine release. Secondly, we grew cultures in the presence of the mitotic inhibitor (1 μM β-d-arabinofuranoside hydrochloride) which reduces glial cell proliferation. Immunostaining of 2–3-week-old cultures revealed a clear reduction in the number of glia in cultures treated with the mitotic inhibitor (Fig. 4c). There was also a significant (P < 0.01) reduction in the amount of purines released from cells cultured in the presence of the mitotic inhibitor (1.18 ± 0.14 μM′ vs 2.96 ± 0.6 μM′, six coverslips with a mitotic inhibiter and six control coverslips, interleaved experiments). This reduction in purine release could specifically reflect the loss of glia or could simply result from a reduction in total cell number if both neurones and glia release purines. In glia-only cultures (confirmed by immunohistochemistry, Fig. 4d), we could detect no purine release in response to KCl (25 mM) and glutamate (1 mM, n = 10) but release was sometimes observed in response to the removal of extracellular Ca2+ (not illustrated). Interpretation of this data is difficult as changes in culture composition may affect differentiation and protein expression. However, it is clear that cultures are mixed (neurones and glia) and there is no obvious change in cell morphology (and presumably cell viability) following stimulation.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Culture composition determines purine release. Culture composition was determined by marking neuronal dendrites with MAP-2 antibodies (green) and marking glia with GFAP antibodies (red). Purine release was evoked by addition of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate (black bar) and monitored with an ADO biosensor and null sensor placed on the culture surface. Example photographs and biosensor traces from: a 21-day culture, b 4-day culture, c 21-day culture with mitotic inhibitor present, d astrocyte only culture. Pictures were taken using an SP2 confocal microscope (single scan). Scale bar for photographs (a, b and c) is 30 μm and 50 μm (d). Note the different morphology of glia when cultured alone compared to morphology when cultured with neurones

Purines release in acute cerebellar slices differs from release in cultures

To investigate whether a similar mechanism of purine release occurs in cerebellar slices, we have applied the same stimulus (25 mM K+ and 1 mM glutamate) to cerebellar slices and measured purine release with biosensors. Although it is not possible to directly compare the age of animals used for slices with the days in culture, we have used slices from two ages of rats (P7–9 and P21–25) to investigate possible changes in purine release during cerebellar development.

In cerebellar slices from P7–9 rats addition of KCl (25 mM) and glutamate (1 mM) produced a large current on the ADO biosensor (449 ± 66 pA, Fig. 5a), with no current on the null sensor (n = 6). This current was equivalent to the release of 2.5 ± 0.2 μM′ adenosine/inosine. We found that the properties of release were similar to those found for release in cultures (Fig. 5b): not blocked by TTX (1 μM, n = 3), Ca2+ dependent (n = 4) and not accompanied by ATP release (n = 6). Blocking conversion of adenosine to inosine with EHNA (20 μM, n = 3) had only a small effect on the current (∼25% reduction) suggesting that the majority of the purine released was not adenosine. However, unlike cultures, purine release from cerebellar slices was not reduced by prolonged incubation (20–30 min) with NBTI (5 μM) and dipyridamole (10 μM, n = 4). Thus, the purine release in slices did not appear to occur via a transporter which is sensitive to NBTI/dipyridamole.

Fig. 5
figure 5

The same stimulus produces purine release in acute cerebellar slices but release is not blocked by ENT inhibition. a Traces from ADO and ATP biosensors placed on the surface of the molecular layer of a cerebellar slice from a P8 rat. Addition of 25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate produced a large current on the ADO biosensor but no signal on the ATP biosensor. b Graph summarising the effects of manipulations (addition of 1 μM TTX, removal of extracellular Ca2+, addition of 20 μM EHNA, addition of 5 μM NBTI and 10 μM dipyridamole) on purine release from slices from P7–9 rats and from P21–23 rats (n = 3–6 for each manipulation). The normalised purine release was calculated by dividing each manipulated (i.e. with zero Ca2+ or with a drug present) purine release current by the average current in control conditions. Data was corrected for differences in biosensor sensitivity across experiments

In cerebellar slices from P21–25 rats addition of KCl (25 mM) and glutamate (1 mM) also produced a large current on the ADO biosensor (299 ± 65 pA, equivalent to 1.46 ± 0.5 μM′ adenosine/inosine, n = 6). Purine release was similar to release in slices from younger rats (Fig. 5b) in that it was insensitive to TTX (1 μM, n = 3), relatively insensitive to EHNA (20 μM, ∼15% reduction, n = 3) and insensitive to NBTI (5 μM) /dipyridamole (10 μM, n = 4). However, unlike purine release in slices from younger rats and in cultures, purine release was not inhibited by the removal of extracellular Ca2+ with no ATP detected on ATP biosensors (n = 4).

Discussion

We have used granule cell cultures to investigate whether the adenosine release observed in cerebellar slices [12] could be from granule cell axons. Although it was not possible to detect the action-potential dependent release of adenosine from granule cell cultures, it was possible to detect the release of adenosine metabolites (inosine/hypoxanthine) in response to a depolarising (stressful) stimulus. Although similar data has been described in other brain areas [7], this is the first time that purine release has been measured from primary cultured cells in real-time using microelectrode biosensors.

There are several possible reasons why the release of adenosine observed in slices was not observed in granule cell cultures. The simplest is that the adenosine release observed in slices is not from granule cells but instead is from a downstream target of parallel fibres and this target is absent from cultures. Such a target could be large cells such as Bergmann glia and Purkinje cells which are not present in the cultures. Alternatively, the cells in cultures are isolated from the cerebella of rats which are 7–8 days old. As it is not possible to record activity-dependent adenosine release in cerebellar slices from P7–10 rats [20], the mechanism of adenosine release may not be present in cultured cells if they do not develop further whilst in culture. Lastly, it could be that the cells (granule cells and glia) present in culture have different properties than those in acute cerebellar slices. The cells are cultured in 25 mM K+ which greatly increases cell survival but may well change their properties, e.g. a greater resting [Ca2+]i may induce widespread gene expression. This possibility is supported by the observation that NBTI/dipyridamole-sensitive purine release that occurs in cultures is not observed in slices. Thus, either a different mechanism releases purines in cultured cells or a different transporter is involved. We have previously observed [20, 21] that in many slices, the efflux of adenosine produced by blocking adenosine kinase (with iodotubercidin) is not inhibited by blocking ENTs with NBTI/dipyridamole. Thus, the cells in slices either have a transporter which is insensitive to NBTI/dipyridamole or release purines by a transporter-independent mechanism. This difference between cultures and slices high lights the problem of extrapolating the mechanisms of purine release between different models.

Mechanisms of purine release in culture

Using a strongly depolarising and stressful stimulus (25 mM KCl and 1 mM glutamate), it was possible to reliably release inosine/hypoxanthine from granule cell cultures. Our data suggests that this stimulus results in the intracellular build-up of either inosine or hypoxanthine inside cells which is then transported from the cell via an NBTI/dipyridamole-sensitive equilibrative transporter (ENT). It is unlikely that the intracellular inosine/hypoxanthine arise from the breakdown of adenosine, as blocking adenosine deaminase with EHNA had no effect on release [22]. Thus presumably intracellular AMP is converted to IMP plus ammonia (by AMP deaminase) and then IMP is metabolised to inosine (by 5′nucleotidases) [23]. The ammonia produced is potentially toxic; for example, there is evidence that it produces swelling of astrocytes following liver failure [24]. The conversion of AMP to IMP predominates in rat C6 glioma cells in ATP depleting conditions [25]. The involvement of AMP deaminase is supported by the observation that the release of inosine/hypoxanthine is Ca2+ dependent. The affinity of AMP deaminase for AMP is increased in the presence of Ca2+ in human red blood cells [26] and Ca2+-calmodulin activates erythrocyte AMP deaminase [27]. The AMP appears to be produced as a result of intracellular ATP metabolism rather than the conversion of adenosine to AMP as blocking adenosine kinase did not result in adenosine release. Studies in cortical neuron/astrocyte cultures [18, 22] have shown that inosine is released in an ENT-dependent mechanism from neurones in response to metabolic stress (oxygen/glucose deprivation or sodium cyanide). The stimulus we have used (KCl and glutamate) will produce an increase in ATP metabolism because of the activation of exchange pumps following prolonged depolarisation and resultant ion movements. The influx of Ca2+ into cells is required for release (maybe to activate AMP deaminase) and this presumably comes via the opening of HVA Ca2+ channels and possibly via glutamate receptor channels.

Why is inosine/hypoxanthine released rather than adenosine? It is possible that the breakdown products of ATP metabolism are expelled from cells and have no signalling role. This seems unlikely as it would result in the loss of purines from the cells. Inosine is suggested to have a signalling role, activating A3 receptors [28]. There is also evidence that inosine/hypoxanthine may play a neurotrophic role [29]. For example, inosine can stimulate morphological and biochemical differentiation in primary cultures of chick sympathetic neurons [30]. Inosine can also enhance the effects of NGF on neurite extension in PC12 cells [31]. The synthetic hypoxanthine derivative AIT-082 enhances the outgrowth of neurites in primary neuronal cultures [32]. Administration of hypoxanthine significantly improved somatosensory evoked potential recovery following ischemic brain injury [33]. The purine metabolite urate is a predictor of decline during Parkinson’s disease, possibly suggesting raised levels of neuroprotective purines [34].

Which cells in culture release purines? This is currently unclear. Reducing the number of glia in cultures reduces purine release and neurone only cultures do not release purines. However, purine release does not occur in glia-only cultures. These results have to be interpreted with caution as the astrocytes cultured without neurones clearly had a different morphology from those cultured with neurones and there was also a difference in neurones grown with reduced glia, neuronal processes were much more blebbed and cell bodies were stained more widely (not illustrated). Thus, the change in growing conditions may well affect the purine release mechanism and thus it is not possible to currently conclude which cells actually release purines in response to stimulation. However, it appears that the interaction between neurones and glia results in one or other (or both) cell types becoming competent to release purines.

The mechanisms of purine release remain controversial and as yet are not fully resolved. This study shows that purine release is highly complex and great care must be used when extrapolating between different stimuli and model systems.