Skip to main content
Log in

Conceptual change within dyadic interactions: the dance of conceptual and material agency

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We offer a new approach to emergent knowledge in processes of conceptual change in dyadic interaction by drawing on Pickering’s (The Mangle of Practice, The University of Chicago Press, London, 1995) Mangle of Practice theory, which theorizes the emergence of new scientific knowledge as occurring due to material resistance and human accommodations to such resistance. We use Commognition (Sfard in Thinking as communicating, Cambridge University Press, New York, 2008), which conceptualizes learning as a change in discourse, and conceptual change as change in meta-discursive rules, to examine the interaction between several dyads working on proportional reasoning tasks. These dyads were selected for close scrutiny based on a large previous experimental study designed to examine the most productive constellation for pairing students of different levels with or without a hypothesis testing device. The Mangle helps us theorize the emergent, unpredictable aspects of learning and conceptual change. We provide three criteria for examining the productivity of the interaction: Availability of an alternative discourse (proportional vs. additive), Resistance of material/disciplinary agency and Positioning of students to make meta-discursive shifts. We discuss this model as an aid for monitoring and designing learning situations that enhance conceptual change.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The study of conceptual change is an extremely vast domain of research. We have here mentioned one camp in this domain (very partially)—the camp of knowledge-as-theory perspective, and totally ignored the knowledge-as-elements perspective. According to the knowledge-as-theory perspective (e.g., Chi 2005; Ioannides and Vosniadou 2002; Wellman and Gelman 1992), naïve knowledge is highly organized (in theory, schema, or frame), coherent, involves the creation of mental models, and its replacement is revolutionary. In contrast, the knowledge-as-elements perspective (e.g., Clark 2006; diSessa 1993) is characterized by naïve knowledge constituted as a collection of quasi-independent knowledge elements, and high contextual sensitivity. Conceptual change according to this perspective involves evolutionary revision and refinement. The explanations involve the p-prims and other elements that are most strongly cued by the context. We intentionally unified the two perspectives in the body, to stress resistance to change, as naïve knowledge is fueled by everyday experience. This simplified presentation enables us to introduce more smoothly the “solution” proposed by the Genevan school to overcome resistance to conceptual change.

  2. For the present task, which includes physical objects (cubes and blocks) that have physical attributes (weight), it may have been necessary to add a fifth characteristic: that of tactical/concrete or physical mediators. However, since eventually this mediator is not discussed in the present analysis, we chose to omit this characteristic from our table.

  3. The quaternions started out as an extension of the complex number system. Hamilton’s goal was to generalize the mapping between algebra and geometry, as can be seen in the mapping of complex numbers to the Cartesian two-dimensional plane, into three-dimensional space. He started out by searching for “triplets” that would map to 3D space in a similar way to x + iy mapping onto the plane. This was done by adding an imaginary “j” and constructing the triplet t = x + iy + jz in the hope that this “j” act in algebraic calculations similarly to the familiar i. However, a series of “resistances”, in the form of geometrical meanings not aligning with the algebraic calculations, prohibited Hamilton from attaining his goal. After a series of attempts to adapt his new system to previous mathematical rules (disciplinary agency), Hamilton’s finally came up with another imaginary: k = ij. He named the set a + ix + jy + kz a “quaternion” and rapidly came up with a series of equations that satisfied both algebraic rules and geometrical meanings. However, with that, Hamilton effectively abandoned his original intention of mapping algebra onto 3 dimensional space. Pickering uses this case as another example in which human intentions are “mangled” with material (or disciplinary) agency, leading to unexpected outcomes and cultural extensions.

  4. Pickering (1995) did not use the term “conceptual agency”, but rather the term “conceptual practice”. His use of the term “concept”, however, seems to agree with that of ours. Following, Sfard (2008 p. 111) we define a concept as “a symbol together with its uses”. Pickering suggests moving away from a “representational idiom” of science, where science is regarded as a representation of reality, to a “performative idiom” of science, where science is “regarded as a field of powers, capacities, and performances, situated in machinic captures of material agency” (p. 7). Though this performative idiom does take into account material agency, the “conceptual” part of science is always regarded by Pickering as social or “cultural” as he terms it, thus being in harmony with a “discursive” definition of concepts.

  5. The dyads’ interactions were initially reported in Cohen-Eliyahu (2011).

  6. A fully proportional correct answer would yield that D was necessarily bigger than C. One possible explanation would be that a B cube is 1.111 times an A cube and therefore 28 C cubes times 1.111 equals 31.11 which is necessarily bigger than 31 D cubes. Algebraically this could be written as 10x < 9y ⇒ 31y < 31.1111y < 28x.

  7. We are aware that the experimenter’s role in this situation could be thought of as critical for the advancement that Dina made, and that this could be seen as strengthening the socio-cultural view insisting on the necessity of an expert’s discourse for the occurrence of conceptual change. However, we claim that this contribution, was, after all, quite minor as it only strengthened existing ideas. Thus, the expert did not play the role of introducing new meta-discursive rules but rather strengthened the position of the students to make the meta-discursive shift. As important as this may be, positional strength (or strong mathematical identities) may be achieved by other means, not just adults’ assurances.

  8. In other domains, such as science education, increasing attention has been given to “hot” conceptual change, including motivation, emotions and beliefs (cf. Sinatra 2014). Similar attention has not been given, to the far of our knowledge to conceptual change in mathematics.

References

  • Asterhan, C. S. C., & Schwarz, B. B. (2007). The effects of monological and dialogical argumentation on concept learning in evolutionary theory. The Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 626–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asterhan, C. S. C., & Schwarz, B. B. (2009). Argumentation and explanation in conceptual change: Indications from protocol analyses of peer-to-peer dialogue. Cognitive Science, 33, 374–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asterhan, C. S. C., Schwarz, B. B., & Cohen-Eliyahu, N. (2014). Outcome feedback during collaborative learning: Contingencies between feedback and dyad composition. Learning and Instruction, 34, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination: Four essays by M. M. Bakhtin. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, J. (2012). “She’s always been the smart one. I’ve always been the dumb one”: Identities in the mathematics classroom. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 43(1), 34–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boaler, J., & Greeno, J. (2000). Identity, agency, and knowing in mathematics worlds. In J. Boaler (Ed.), Multiple perspectives on mathematics education (pp. 171–200). Westport, CT: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, T. W., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Development of proportional reasoning: Where young children go wrong. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1478–1490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, N. J. S., Danish, J. A., Levin, M., & diSessa, A. A. (2016). Competence reconceived—the shared enterprise of knowledge analysis and interaction analysis. In A. A. diSessa, M. Levin, & N. J. S. Brown (Eds.), Knowledge and interaction—a synthetic agenda for the learning sciences. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1992). The origin and evolution of everyday concepts. In R. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science (pp. 89–128). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(2), 161–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2008). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), Handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 61–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of Anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. B. (2006). Longitudinal conceptual change in students’ understanding of thermal equilibrium: An examination of the process of conceptual restructuring. Cognition and Instruction, 24(4), 467–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen-Eliyahu, N. (2011). Prerequisites for successful interaction: Cognitive configuration of dyads and hypothesis testing. Unpublished dissertation, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem.

  • de Leeuw, N., & Chi, M. T. H. (2003). Self-explanation: Enriching a situation model or repairing a domain model? In G. Sinatra & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Intentional conceptual change (pp. 55–78). Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10(2–3), 105–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A. (2014). A history of conceptual change research: Threads and fault lines. In K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (2nd ed., pp. 88–108). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A., Levin, M., & Brown, N. J. S. (2016). Knowledge and interaction: A synthetic agenda for the learning sciences. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1979). Individual and collective conflicts of centration in cognitive development. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9(1), 245–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1984). The social development of the intellect. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., Mugny, G., & Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (1975). Social interaction and the development of logical operations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 6(3), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. A., & Conant, F. R. (2002). Guiding principles for fostering productive diciplinary engagement: Explaining an emergent argument in a community of learners classroom. Cognition and Instruction, 20(4), 399–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glachan, M., & Light, P. (1982). Peer interaction and learning: Can two wrongs make a right? In G. Butterworth & P. Light (Eds.), Social cognition: Studies in the development of understanding (pp. 238–262). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannula, M. (2012). Exploring new dimensions of mathematics-related affect: Embodied and social theories. Research in Mathematics Education, 14(2), 137–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, G., Behr, M., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1992). The Blocks task: Comparative analyses of the task with other proportion tasks and qualitative reasoning skills of sevengrade children in solving tasks. Cognition and Instruction, 9(1), 45–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R., & van Langenhove, L. (1999). Positioning theory. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbel-Eisenmann, B. A., Wagner, D., Johnson, K. R., Suh, H., & Figueras, H. (2015). Positioning in mathematics education: Revelations on an imported theory. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 89, 185–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyd-Metzuyanim, E. (2013). The co-construction of learning difficulties in mathematics—teacher–student interactions and their role in the development of a disabled mathematical identity. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 83(3), 341–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyd-Metzuyanim, E. (2015). Vicious cycles of identifying and mathematizing—a case study of the development of mathematical failure. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 24(4), 504–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyd-Metzuyanim, E., & Sfard, A. (2012). Identity struggles in the mathematics classroom: On learning mathematics as an interplay of mathematizing and identifying. International Journal of Educational Research, 51–52, 128–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, C. (2009). Collaborative group work in middle childhood: Joint construction, unresolved contradiction and the growth of knowledge. Human Development, 52(4), 215–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2002). The changing meaning of force. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 2, 5–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Limón, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: A critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11, 357–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L. (2007). Introduction: Bridging the cognitive and sociocultural approaches in research on conceptual change: Is it feasible? Educational Psychologist, 42(1), 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S., & Brownell, C. (1975). Peers, persuasion and Piaget: Dyadic interaction between conservers and non-conservers. Child Development, 46(4), 992–997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mix, K., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. (2002). Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: Is number one of them? Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 278–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nachlieli, T., & Tabach, M. (2012). Growing mathematical objects in the classroom—The case of function. International Journal of Educational Research, 51–52, 10–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noelting, G. (1980). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept: Part 2—problem structure at successive stages: Problem solving strategies and the mechanism of adaptive restructuring. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11, 331–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origins of the idea of chance in children. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, A. (1995). The Mangle of practice. London: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoultz, J., Säljö, R., & Wyndham, J. (2001). Heavenly talk: Discourse, artifacts, and children’s understanding of elementary astronomy. Human Development, 44, 103–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., & Linchevski, L. (2007). The role of task design and of argumentation in cognitive development during peer interaction. The case of proportional reasoning. Learning and Instruction, 17(5), 510–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., Neuman, Y., & Biezuner, S. (2000). Two “wrongs” may make a right…If they argue together! Cognition & Instruction, 18(4), 461–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2007a). Reconceptualizing conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou, A. Baltas, & X. Vamvakoussi (Eds.), Re-framing the conceptual change in learning and instruction (pp. 331–337). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2007b). When the rules of discourse change, but nobody tells you: Making sense of mathematics learning from a commognitive standpoint. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 16(4), 565–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2008). Thinking as communicating. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2009). Commentary on the chapters by Baker and Asterhan and Schwarz through the lens of commognition. In B. B. Schwarz, T. Dreyfus, & R. Hershkowitz (Eds.), Transformation of knowledge through classroom interaction (pp. 173–183). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A., & Prusak, A. (2005). Telling identities: In search of an analytic tool for investigating learning as a culturally shaped activity. Educational Researcher, 34(4), 14–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 481–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G. M. (2014). The “warming trend” in conceptual change research: The legacy of Paul R. Pintrich. Educational Psychologist, 1520(August), 37–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. A., & Resnick, L. B. (1992). Representations of proportional relationships: Are children part–part or part–whole reasoners? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 23(3), 231–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer-Freeman, K. E., & Goswami, U. (2001). Does a half pizza equal half a box of chocolate? Proportional matching in an analogy task. Cognitive Development, 16, 811–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1999). Conceptual change research: State of the art and future directions. In W. Schnotz, S. Vosniadou, & M. Carretero (Eds.), New perspectives on conceptual change (pp. 3–13). Amsterdam: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, M. B., & Kalinec, C. A. (2012). Student talk and opportunities for mathematical learning in small group interactions. International Journal of Educational Research, 51–52, 109–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wortham, S. (2005). Learning identity: The joint emergence of social identification and academic learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Yamakawa, Y., Forman, E., & Ansell, E. (2009). Role of positioning: The role of positioning in constructing an identity in a third grad mathematics classroom. In K. Kumpulainen, C. E. Hmelo-Silver, & M. César (Eds.), Investigating classroom interaction: Methodologies in action (pp. 179–202). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The writing of this paper was partially supported by the Golda Meir postdoctoral fellowship of the Hebrew University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Einat Heyd-Metzuyanim.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Heyd-Metzuyanim, E., Schwarz, B.B. Conceptual change within dyadic interactions: the dance of conceptual and material agency. Instr Sci 45, 645–677 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-017-9419-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-017-9419-z

Keywords

Navigation