Skip to main content
Log in

Gender Roles and Empathic Accuracy: The Role of Communion in Reading Minds

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although empathic accuracy is considered a stable skill, few individual difference measures consistently predict performance on Ickes’ (e.g., 2001) empathic accuracy measure. Because past work has shown that women are more empathically accurate than men when female gender roles are made salient before an empathic accuracy task, we hypothesized that self-reported communion and related variables might predict empathic accuracy. Participants (194 undergraduates) from a northwestern U.S. university completed an empathic accuracy task and self-report measures of communion and empathy. Communion and empathic concern predicted greater empathic accuracy, but only after controlling for socially desirable responding. The role of communion in empathic inference is discussed, along with the need to include measures of social desirability when examining correlates of empathic accuracy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buysse, A., & Ickes, W. (1999). Topic-relevant cognition and empathic accuracy in laboratory discussions of safer sex. Psychology & Health, 14, 351–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constantine, M. G. (2000). Social desirability attitudes, sex, and affective and cognitive empathy as predictors of self-reported multicultural counseling competence. The Counseling Psychologist, 28, 857–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curwen, T. (2003). The importance of offense characteristics, victimization history, hostility, and social desirability in assessing empathy of male adolescent sex offenders. Sexual Abuse, 15, 347–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1997). Personality and empathic accuracy. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 144–168). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foushee, H. C., Helmreich, R. L., & Spence, J. T. (1979). Implicit theories of masculinity and femininity: dualistic or bipolar? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 3, 259–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gesn, P. R., & Ickes, W. (1999). The development of meaning contexts for empathic accuracy: channel and sequence effects.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 746–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women’s intuition isn’t greater than men’s. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 117–143). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1980). Masculinity and femininity in children: development of the children’s Personal Attributes Questionnaire. Developmental Psychology, 16, 270–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Schmid Mast, M. (2007). Sources of accuracy in the empathic accuracy paradigm. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 7, 438–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helgeson, V. S. (1993). Implications of agency and communion for patient and spouse adjustment to a first coronary event. Added. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 807–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helgeson, V. S. (1994). Relation of agency and communion to well-being: evidence and potential explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 412–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, S. D., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2007). Balancing the empathy expense account: strategies for regulating empathic response. In T. Farrow, & P. Woodruff (Eds.), Empathy in mental illness (pp. 389–407). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, S. D., & Wegner, D. M. (1997). Automatic and controlled empathy. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic Accuracy (pp. 311–339). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horgan, T. G., & Smith, J. L. (2006). Interpersonal reasons for interpersonal perceptions: gender-incongruent purpose goals and nonverbal judgment accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 127–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (1997). Introduction. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic Accuracy (pp. 1–16). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W. (2001). Measuring empathic accuracy. In J. Hall, & F. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: theory, measurement and applications (pp. 219–241). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Bissonnette, V., Garcia, S., & Stinson, L. L. (1990a). Implementing and using the dyadic interaction paradigm. In C. Hendrick, & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Review of personality and social psychology: vol. 11. Research methods in personality and social psychology (pp. 16–44). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Stinson, L., Bissonnette, V., & Garcia, S. (1990b). Naturalistic social cognition: empathic accuracy in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 730–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Buysse, A., Pham, H., Rivers, K., Erickson, J. R., Hancock, M., et al. (2000a). On the difficulty of distinguishing ‘good’ and ‘poor’ perceivers: a social relations analysis of empathic accuracy data. Personal Relationships, 7, 219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., & Graham, T. (2000b). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: differential ability or differential motivation? Personal Relationships, 7, 219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaccard, J., & Turrisi, R. (2003). Interaction effects in multiple regression (2nd ed.). Sage University Papers Series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, 07-072. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • John, O. P., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Measurement: reliability, construct validation, and scale construction. In H. T. Reis, & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality and social psychology (pp. 339–369). New York: Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, K. J. K., & Hodges, S. D. (2001). Gender differences, motivation, and empathic accuracy: when it pays to understand. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 720–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2005). Stereotype threat in men on a test of social sensitivity. Sex Roles, 52, 489–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, A. G. (1982). Psychological androgyny and social desirability. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 147–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lönnqvist, J. E., Verkasalo, M., & Bezmenova, I. (2007). Agentic and communal bias in socially desirable responding. European Journal of Personality, 21, 853–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marangoni, C., Garcia, S., Ickes, W., & Teng, G. (1995). Empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 854–869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Antill, J. K., & Cunningham, J. D. (1987). Masculinity, femininity, and androgyny: relations to self-esteem and social desirability. Journal of Personality, 55, 661–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Antill, J. K., & Cunningham, J. D. (1989). Masculinity and femininity: a bipolar construct and independent constructs. Journal of Personality, 57, 625–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 424–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miville, M. L., Gelso, C. J., Pannu, R., Liu, W., Touradji, P., Holloway, P., et al. (1999). Appreciating similarities and valuing differences: the Miville–Guzman universality-diversity scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 291–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, M. W., & Hodges, S. D.(in press). Making it up and making do: simulation, imagination and empathic accuracy. To appear in K. Markman, W. Klein, & J. Suhr (Eds.), The handbook of imagination and mental simulation. New York: Psychology.

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—what others know: imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 737–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. In J. P. Robinson, P. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 17–59). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (2002). Socially desirable responding: the evolution of a construct. In H. I. Braun, D. N. Jackson, & D. E. Wiley (Eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and educational measurement (pp. 49–69). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L., & Reid, D. H. (1991). Enhancement and denial in socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research: explanation and prediction, 3 rd Ed. London: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: the contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schenk, J., & Heinsch, R. (1986). Self-descriptions by means of sex-role scales and personality scales: a critical evaluation of recent masculinity and femininity scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 161–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skoe, E. E., Cumberland, A., Eisenberg, N., Hansen, K., & Perry, J. (2002). The influences of sex and gender-role identity on moral cognition and prosocial personality traits. Sex Roles, 46, 295–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T. (1984). Masculinity, femininity, and gender-related traits: a conceptual analysis and critique of current research. In B. A. Maher, & W. Maher (Eds.), Progress in experimental research (vol. Vol. 13, (pp. 2–97)). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T. (1991). Do the BSRI and PAQ measure the same or different concepts? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 141–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T. (1993). Gender-related traits and gender ideology: evidence for a multifactorial theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 624–635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. E. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits, trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes: what do they signify? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 44–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal Attributes Questionnaire: a measure of sex role stereotypes and masculinity–femininity. JSAS: Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 4, 43–44 (Ms. No. 617).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stinson, L., & Ickes, W. (1992). Empathic accuracy in the interaction of male friends versus male strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 787–797.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, D. (1981). Social desirability and the Bem Sex Role Inventory. Psychological Reports, 48, 503–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, G., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (2003). Mind-reading accuracy in intimate relationships: assessing the relationship of the relationship, the target, and the judge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1079–1094.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. (2008). It takes two: the interpersonal nature of empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 19, 399–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Jennifer Freyd, Adam Kramer, Karyn Lewis, John Myers, Mike Myers, Rebecca Neel, and Gerard Saucier for their insightful comments on an earlier draft of this paper. We would also like to thank Tim Mathews for serving as the experimenter and Carlie Ashcraft, Mary D’Anna, John Meyers, Rebecca Neel, Carissa Sharp, and Liana Vega for their help in coding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean M. Laurent.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Laurent, S.M., Hodges, S.D. Gender Roles and Empathic Accuracy: The Role of Communion in Reading Minds. Sex Roles 60, 387–398 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9544-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9544-x

Keywords

Navigation