Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Interpersonal Reasons for Interpersonal Perceptions: Gender-incongruent Purpose Goals and Nonverbal Judgment Accuracy

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Women’s tendency to outperform men on measures of accuracy in interpreting the meaning of nonverbal behavior might be due to such measures being more congruent with women’s interpersonal goals than men’s. The present study examined undergraduate men’s and women’s (N = 41) nonverbal judgment accuracy on the Interpersonal Perception Task-15 (IPT-15; Costanzo & Archer, 1993 [.The interpersonal perception task-15 (IPT-15). Berkeley: University of California Center for Media and Independent Learning) when the purpose for using their judgment skills was manipulated to be either congruent or incongruent with stereotypic “masculine” and “feminine” interpersonal goals. Results showed that each gender was at a relative disadvantage in judgment accuracy in the gender-incongruent goal conditions: women were relatively less accurate when they thought the IPT-15 measured judgment skills of use to interrogators in the military, whereas men were relatively less accurate when they thought the IPT-15 measured judgment skills of use to social workers in the social services. Discussion centers on the importance of matching individuals’ interpersonal goals to the purpose goals of the measure when using measures of interpersonal sensitivity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. An ANOVA on men’s and women’s anticipated interest scores in only the 2 purpose goal conditions did not reveal a significant Gender × Condition interaction (p = .17).

  2. An ANOVA on men’s and women’s task-absorption scores in only the 2 purpose goal conditions did not reveal a significant Gender × Condition interaction (F < 1).

  3. Exploratory correlations (Pearson r) showed that participants’ anticipated interest in doing the IPT-15 was positively correlated with their interest in doing another set of judgment tasks (i.e., future interest), r(134) = .40, p < .001, expected performance on the IPT-15, r(134) = .26, p = .003, and feelings of task absorption while taking the IPT-15, r(134) = .34, p < .001. Also, participants’ task-absorption scores were positively correlated with their future-interest scores, r(134) = .33, p < .001.

References

  • Ames, D. R., & Kammrath, L. K. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism, not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 187–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, G., & Wilkes, J. M. (1989). A cognitive interpretation of the marginality and underrepresentation of women in science. Journal of Higher Education, 60, 371–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80, 706–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bianco, A. T., Higgins, E. T., & Klem, A. (2003). How “fun/importance” fit affects performance: Relating implicit theories to instructions. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1091–1103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briton, N. J., & Hall, J. A. (1995). Beliefs about female and male nonverbal communication. Sex Roles, 32, 79–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cataldi, A. E., & Reardon, R. (1996). Gender, interpersonal orientation, and manipulation tactic use in close relationships. Sex Roles, 35, 205–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behavior of others: The interpersonal perception task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 225–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1993). The Interpersonal Perception Task-15 (IPT-15), Berkeley: University of California Center for Media and Independent Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, T., & Ickes, W. (1997). When women’s intuition isn’t greater than men’s. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 117–143). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (2001). The PONS test and the psychometric approach to measuring interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall, & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 143–160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall J. A., & Bernieri F. J. (Eds.) (2001). Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement. Mahwah, NJ: Erlhaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there from here. In M. L. Maehr, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.) Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 21–49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helgeson, V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (1998). A theory of unmitigated communion. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 2, 173–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horgan, T. G., Schmid Mast, M., Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (2004). Gender differences in memory for the appearance of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 185–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., & Graham, T. (2000). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: Differential ability or differential motivation? Personal Relationships, 7, 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iizuka, Y., Patterson, M. L., & Machen, J. C. (2002). Accuracy and confidence on the interpersonal perception task: A Japanese–American comparison. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 159–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaac, J., Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (1999). Other people as a source of interest in an activity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 239–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M. F., Schaefer, E. G., & Zinkiewicz, L. (1994). Member preference for discussion content in anticipated group decisions: Effects of type of issue and group interactive goal. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 15, 489–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, K. J. K., & Hodges, S. D. (2001). Gender differences, motivation, and empathic accuracy: When it pays to understand. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 720–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers. Oxford, England: Harpers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leyens, J.-P., Desert, M., Croizet, J.-C., & Darcis, C. (2000). Stereotype threat: Are lower status and history of stigmatization preconditions of stereotype threat? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1189–1199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C., Isaac, J. D., & Sansone, C. (2001). The role of interest in understanding the career choices of female and male college students. Sex Roles, 44, 295–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mufson, L., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (1991). Factors affecting the accuracy of facial affect recognition. The Journal of Social Psychology, 131, 815–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Hartigan, M. (1987). Accuracy of facial affect recognition as a function of locus of control orientation and anticipated interpersonal interaction. The Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 363–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., & Richman, D. (1985). The effect of standard, motivation, and strategy instructions on the facial processing accuracy of internal and external subjects. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 354–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosip, J. C., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Knowledge of nonverbal cues, gender, and nonverbal decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 268–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). “I don’t feel like it”: The function of interest in self-regulation. In L. L. Martin, & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 203–228). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Morgan, C. (1992). Intrinsic motivation and education: Competence in context. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 249–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., Sachau, D. A., & Weir, C. (1989). Effects of instruction on intrinsic interest: The importance of context. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57, 819–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). The “how” of goal pursuit: Interest and self-regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 306–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, I. G. (1980). Introduction to the study of test anxiety. In I.G. Sarason (Ed.), Test anxiety: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 3–14). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50, 835–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. L. (2004). Understanding the process of stereotype threat: A review of mediational variables and new performance goal directions. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 177–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. L., & Johnson, C. S. (2006). A stereotype boost or chocking under pressure? Positive gender stereotypes and men who are low in domain identification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 51–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. L., Morgan, C. L., & Sansone, C. (2001). Getting (inter) personal: The role of other people in the self-regulation of interest. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. V). Huntington, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nullified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s not just a woman’s issue. Sex Roles, 47, 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African-Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strough, J., Berg, C. A., & Sansone, C. (1996). Goals for solving everyday problems across the life span: Age and gender differences in the salience of interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1106–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swap, W. C., & Rubin, J. Z. (1983). Measurement of interpersonal orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 208–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, G., Fletcher, G. J. O., & Lange, C. (1997). On-line empathic accuracy in marital interaction. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72, 839–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timmers, M., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Gender differences in motives for regulating emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 974–985.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Grant No. T32-MH19728 from the National Institute of Mental Health to the first author. The authors wish to thank Brittany LaFuse, Sandra Lee, Jessica Peugh, Tanya Patel, Katie Riggle, Daveen Ruiz, and T. Ryan Snyder for their assistance with data collection and analysis, and Judith Hall, Marianne McGrath, and Marianne Schmid Mast for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Terrence G. Horgan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Horgan, T.G., Smith, J.L. Interpersonal Reasons for Interpersonal Perceptions: Gender-incongruent Purpose Goals and Nonverbal Judgment Accuracy. J Nonverbal Behav 30, 127–140 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-006-0012-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-006-0012-4

Keywords

Navigation