Skip to main content
Log in

First-Date Scripts: Gender Roles, Context, and Relationship

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study sought to discover whether variations in relevant features of a first date would produce differences in young adults’ first-date scripts. In responding to scenarios that manipulated gender of the date initiator, alcohol availability, and relationship type, 209 college students in the Midwestern United States generated lists including an average of 21 actions they expected to occur on the date. Findings showed a trend toward traditional gender roles for male and female date partners, though some complexity related to sexual behavior and the gender of the date initiator was found. In addition, the context of the date influenced the date script to a great extent, whereas the type of relationship between the date partners had little effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbey, A. (1987). Misperceptions of friendly behavior as sexual interest: A survey of naturally occurring incidents. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 173–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, C. R. (1997). Planning strategic interaction: Attaining goals through strategic interaction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Nonverbal signals. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (2nd ed., pp. 229–284). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1987). Validation and measurement of the fundamental themes of relational communication. Communication Monographs, 54, 19–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. L., Shaver, P. R., & Abrahams, M. F. (1999). Strategic behaviors in romantic relationship initiation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 707–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganong, L. H., & Coleman, M. (1992). Gender differences in expectations of self and future partner. Journal of Family Issues, 13, 55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganong, L. H., Coleman, M., Thompson, A., & Goodwin-Watkins, C. (1996). African American and European American college students’ expectations for self and for future partners. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 758–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, W. H., & Norris, J. (1991). Alcohol, disinhibition, sexual arousal, and deviant sexual behavior. Alcohol Health and Research World, 15, 133–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honeycutt, J. M., & Cantrill, J. G. (2001). Cognition, communication, and romantic relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honeycutt, J. M., Cantrill, J. G., & Greene, R. W. (1989). Memory structures for relational escalation: A cognitive test of sequencing of relational actions and stages. Human Communication Research, 16, 62–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laner, M. R., & Ventrone, N. A. (1998). Egalitarian daters/traditionalist dates. Journal of Family Issues, 19, 468–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, J. R., & Grossman, K. (1991). Initiation of dates and anxiety among college men and women. Psychological Reports, 69, 252–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mongeau, P. A., & Carey, C. M. (1996). Who’s wooing whom II: An experimental investigation of date-initiation and expectancy violation. Western Journal of Communication, 60, 195–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mongeau, P. A., Hale, J. L., Johnson, K. L., & Hillis, J. D. (1993). Who’s wooing whom? An investigation of female-initiated dating. In P. J. Kalbfleisch (Ed.), Interpersonal communication: Evolving interpersonal relationships (pp. 51–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mongeau, P. A., & Johnson, K. L. (1995). Predicting cross-sex first-date sexual expectations and involvement: Contextual and individual difference factors. Personal Relationships, 2, 301–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mongeau, P. A., Morr Serewicz, M. C., & Therrien, L. F. (2004). Goals for cross-sex first dates: Identification, measurement, and the impact of contextual factors. Communication Monographs, 61, 121–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mongeau, P. A., & Teubner, G. (2002, November). Romantic relationship transitions. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, New Orleans, LA.

  • Morr, M. C., & Mongeau, P. A. (2004). First date expectations: The role of sex of initiator, alcohol consumption, and relationship type. Communication Research, 31, 3–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morr Serewicz, M. C., & Stow, C. A. H. (2005, November). Brief report: Stages of romantic relationships at the University of Denver. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Boston, MA.

  • Muehlenhard, C. L., Friedman, D. E., & Thomas, C. M. (1985). Is date rape justifiable? The effects of dating activity, who initiated, who paid, and men’s attitudes toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 9, 297–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muehlenhard, C. L., & Miller, E. N. (1988). Traditional and nontraditional men’s responses to women’s dating initiation. Behavior Modification, 12, 385–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, J. B., & Merluzzi, T. V. (1985). The role of expertise in processing social interaction scripts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 362–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S., & Frieze, I. H. (1989). Young singles’ scripts for a first date. Gender and Society, 3, 258–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S., & Frieze, I. H. (1993). Young singles’ contemporary dating scripts. Sex Roles, 28, 499–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weschler, H., Davenport, A., Dowdall, G., Moeykens, B., & Castillo, S. (1994). Health and behavioral consequences of binge drinking in college. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 1672–1677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

The authors thank Suzanna Rose and Irene H. Frieze for helpful advice regarding data analysis and Paul Mongeau for helpful comments on a previous draft.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mary Claire Morr Serewicz.

Appendix

Appendix

Examples of experimental scenarios manipulating gender of date initiator, alcohol availability, and relationship type:

Example 1. Scenario with male initiator, no alcohol availability, and acquaintance condition

Dan and Jane are sitting together at the Food Court in Shriver Center waiting for the rest of their communication group to show up for a meeting. They are working together with four other people on a group project for their public relations class. Because the semester has just begun, this is their group’s first meeting.

Dan and Jane first met just before winter break when a mutual friend introduced them. They didn’t see each [sic] over the break and because of the hectic nature of the new semester, they have only talked briefly since classes began. On this particular day, they have both arrived early for the group meeting. As they wait for the other group members, they talk about current events on and off campus and end up talking about movies. After discussing a particular new movie, Dan says, “That movie is uptown at the Princess. If you’re not busy this weekend, would you like to go see it with me, and then go to the coffee shop afterward?”

Example 2. Scenario with female initiator, alcohol availability, and friend condition

Dan and Jane are sitting together at the Food Court in Shriver Center waiting for the rest of their communication group to show up for a meeting. They are working together with four other people on a group project for their public relations class. Because the semester has just begun, this is their group’s first meeting.

Dan and Jane know each other very well and have remained close friends since they first met during their freshman year in high school. They are attracted to each other, but have never had the opportunity to act on those feelings because one or the other has always been involved with someone else. For the first time since they met, neither Dan nor Jane is involved with another person. On this particular day, they have both arrived early for the group meeting. As they wait for the other group members, they talk about current events on and off campus and end up talking about movies. After discussing a particular new movie, Jane says, “That movie is uptown at the Princess. If you’re not busy this weekend, would you like to go see it with me, and then go to a keg party at my friends’ house afterward?”

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Morr Serewicz, M.C., Gale, E. First-Date Scripts: Gender Roles, Context, and Relationship. Sex Roles 58, 149–164 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9283-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9283-4

Keywords

Navigation