Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Strategies of Modeling in Biology Education

  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Modeling, like inquiry more generally, is not a single method, but rather a complex suite of strategies. Philosophers of biology, citing the diverse aims, interests, and disciplinary cultures of biologists, argue that modeling is best understood in the context of its epistemic aims and cognitive payoffs. In the science education literature, modeling has been discussed in a variety of ways, but often without explicit reference to the diversity of roles models play in scientific practice. We aim to expand and bring clarity to the myriad uses of models in science by presenting a framework from philosopher of biology Jay Odenbaugh that describes five pragmatic strategies of model use in the biological sciences. We then present illustrative examples of each of these roles from an empirical study of an undergraduate biological modeling curriculum, which highlight how students used models to help them frame their research question, explore ideas, and refine their conceptual understanding in an educational setting. Our aim is to begin to explicate the definition of modeling in science in a way that will allow educators and curriculum developers to make informed choices about how and for what purpose modeling enters science classrooms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Readers interested in an example from physics might want to consider Hughes’ (1999) description of the Ising model, a model that is not faithful to reality but nevertheless has explanatory utility in physics.

  2. See also Harre (1986) for his discussion of models that are used to explore “possibilities” and “impossibilities”.

  3. Prediction in this context is used to refer to the practice of predicting future events with some accuracy. This is different from the reasoning strategy of imagining the implications if a given model were true, often also referred to as the predictions of a model. This type of reasoning is more closely related to exploring possibilities as discussed in Sect. 1.2.2.

  4. All names are pseudonyms.

References

  • Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (2005). Explanation: A mechanist alternative. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 36(2), 421–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (2010). Complex biological mechanisms: Cyclic, oscillatory, and autonomous. Handbook of the Philosophy of Complex Systems, 10, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C., & Malhotra, B. (2002). Epistemologically authentic inquiry in schools: A theoretical framework for evaluating inquiry tasks. Science Education, 86(2), 175–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (1989). Learning via model construction and criticism. In G. Glover, R. Ronning, & C. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity: Assessment, theory and research (pp. 341–381). New York: Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. C. (2000). Model based learning as a key research area for science education. International Journal of Science Education, 22(9), 1041–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G. J. (2003). The science of the struggle for existence: On the foundations of ecology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darden, L. (1991). Theory change in science: Strategies from Mendelian genetics. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darden, L. (2002). Strategies for discovering mechanisms: Schema instantiation, modular subassembly, forward/backward chaining. Philosophy of Science, 69(S3), S354–S365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (2004). Metarepresentation: Native competence and targets for instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(3), 293–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downes, S. (1992). The importance of models in theorizing: A deflationary semantic view. In PSA: Proceedings of the biennial meeting of the philosophy of science association (Vol. 1, pp. 142–153). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

  • Ford, M. J. (2008). Disciplinary authority and accountability in scientific practice and learning. Science Education, 92(3), 404–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. J., & Forman, E. A. (2011). Redefining disciplinary learning in classroom contexts. Educational Research, 30(2006), 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (1988). Explaining science: A cognitive approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (2004). How models are used to represent reality. Philosophy of Science, 71, 742–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobert, J. D. (2005). The effects of different learning tasks on model-building in plate tectonics: Diagramming versus explaining. Journal of Geoscience Education, 53(4), 444–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P. (2006). The strategy of model-based science. Biology and Philosophy, 21(5), 725–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grandy, R., & Duschl, R. (2007). Reconsidering the character and role of inquiry in school science: Analysis of a conference. Science & Education, 16, 141–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosslight, L., Unger, C., Jay, E., & Smith, C. L. (1991). Understanding models and their use in science: Conceptions of middle and high school students and experts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 799–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, D., Russ, R., Mikeska, J., & Scherr, R. (2008). Identifying inquiry and conceptualizing students’ abilities. In R. Duschl & R. Grandy (Eds.), Teaching scientific inquiry (pp. 138–156). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harre, R. (1986). Varieties of realism: A rationale for the natural sciences. New York: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A., & Treagust, D. (2000). A typology of school science models. International Journal of Science Education, 22(9), 1011–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodson, D. (1996). Laboratory work as scientific method: Three decades of confusion and distortion. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 28(2), 115–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodson, D. (1998). Science fiction: The continuing misrepresentation of science in the school curriculum. Pedagogy, Culture and Society, 6(2), 191–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, R. I. G. (1999). The Ising model, computer simulation, and universal physics. In M. Morrison & M. S. Morgan (Eds.), Models as mediators: Perspectives on natural and social science (pp. 97–145). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, E. F. (2000). Models of and models for: Theory and practice in contemporary biology. Philosophy of Science, 67(S1), S72–S86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knorr-Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: How the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knuuttila, T. (2005). Models, representation, and mediation. Philosophy of Science, 72(5), 1260–1271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koponen, I. (2007). Models and modelling in physics education: A critical re-analysis of philosophical underpinnings and suggestions for revisions. Science & Education, 16(7), 751–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1990). Drawing things together. In M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 19–68). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laubichler, M. D., & Müller, G. B. (2007). Modeling biology: Structures, behavior, evolution. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2005). Developing modeling and argument in the elementary grades. In T. A. Rombert, T. P. Carpenter, & F. Dremock (Eds.), Understanding mathematics and science matters (Part II: Learning with understanding). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2006). Cultivating model-based reasoning in science education. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 371–388). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., Schauble, L., & Lucas, D. (2008). Supporting development of the epistemology of inquiry. Cognitive Development, 23, 512–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levins, R. (1966). The strategy of model building in population biology. American Scientist, 54(4), 421–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, E. A. (1994). The structure and confirmation of evolutionary theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machamer, P., Darden, L., & Craver, C. (2000). Thinking about mechanisms. Philosophy of Science, 67(1), 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnani, L., Nersessian, N. J., & Thagard, P. (Eds.). (1999). Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery. New York: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, M. (1994). Science teaching: The role of history and philosophy of science. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, R. (1973). The stability and complexity of model ecosystems. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metz, K. (2010). Children’s understanding of scientific inquiry: Their conceptualization of uncertainty in investigations of their own design. Cognition and Instruction, 22(2), 219–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, M., & Morgan, M. (1999). Models as mediating instruments. In M. Morrison & M. Morgan (Eds.), Models as mediators: Perspectives on natural and social science (pp. 10–37). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1992). How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. In R. N. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science (pp. 3–44). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1999). Model-based reasoning in conceptual change. In L. Magnani, N. Nersessian, & P. Thagard (Eds.), Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery. New York: Kluwer/Plenum Publishers.

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2002). The cognitive basis of model-based reasoning. The cognitive basis of science (pp. 133–153). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2008). Model-based reasoning in scientific practice. In R. Duschl & R. Grandy (Eds.), Teaching Scientific Inquiry: Recommendations for Research and Implementation (pp. 57–79). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

  • NRC. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in grades K-8. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odenbaugh, J. (2005). Idealized, inaccurate but successful: A pragmatic approach to evaluating models in theoretical ecology. Biology and Philosophy, 20(2–3), 231–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odenbaugh, J. (2009). Models in biology. In E. Craig (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osbeck, L., Nersessian, N. J., Malone, K. R., & Newstetter, W. (2010). Science as psychology: Sense-making and identity in science practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pluta, W. J., Chinn, C. A., & Duncan, R. G. (2011). Learners’ epistemic criteria for good scientific models. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(5), 486–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, J. (2005). Epistemology for the masses: The origins of “The Scientific Method” in American schools. History of Education Quarterly, 45(3), 341–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, C., Reiser, B., Davis, E., Kenyon, L., Acher, A., Fortus, D., et al. (2009). Developing a learning progression for scientific modeling: Making scientific modeling accessible and meaningful for learners. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46(6), 632–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, C., & White, B. (2005). Metamodeling knowledge: Developing students’ understanding of scientific modeling. Cognition and Instruction, 23(2), 165–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E., Haarer, S., & Confrey, J. (1997). Seeking diversity in mathematics education: Mathematical modeling in the practice of biologists and mathematicians. Science & Education, 6, 441–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tang, X., Coffey, J., Elby, A., & Levin, D. (2010). The scientific method and scientific inquiry: Tensions in teaching and learning. Science Education, 94(1), 29–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, B. Y. (1993). ThinkerTools: Causal models, conceptual change, and science education. Cognition and instruction, 10(1), 1–100.

  • Wimsatt, W. C. (1987). False models as means to truer theories. In M. Nitecki (Ed.), Neutral models in biology (pp. 23–55). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt, W. C. (2002). Using false models to elaborate constraints on processes: Blending inheritance in organic and cultural evolution. Philosophy of Science, 69(s3), S12–S24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windschitl, M., Thompson, J., & Braaten, M. (2008a). Beyond the scientific method: Model-based inquiry as a new paradigm of preference for school science investigations. Science Education, 1–27. doi:10.1002/sce.

  • Windschitl, M., Thompson, J., & Braaten, M. (2008b). How novice science teachers appropriate epistemic discourses around model-based inquiry for use in classrooms. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 310–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynne, C., Stewart, J., & Passmore, C. (2001). High school students’ use of meiosis when solving genetics problems. International Journal of Science Education, 23(5), 501–515.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the National Science Foundation Interdisciplinary Training for Undergraduates in Biological and Mathematical Sciences (UBM) program under Grant No. 0531935. We would like to acknowledge CLIMB mentors and the students of the 2008–2009 CLIMB cohort for allowing us to bare witness to their learning process. We also thank Richard Lehrer and four anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions for improving this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julia Svoboda.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Svoboda, J., Passmore, C. The Strategies of Modeling in Biology Education. Sci & Educ 22, 119–142 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-011-9425-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-011-9425-5

Keywords

Navigation