Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Implications of the Cognitive Sciences for the Relation Between Religion and Science Education: The Case of Evolutionary Theory

  • Published:
Science & Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper discusses the relationship between religion and science education in the light of the cognitive sciences. We challenge the popular view that science and religion are compatible, a view that suggests that learning and understanding evolutionary theory has no effect on students’ religious beliefs and vice versa. We develop a cognitive perspective on how students manage to reconcile evolutionary theory with their religious beliefs. We underwrite the claim developed by cognitive scientists and anthropologists that religion is natural because it taps into people’s intuitive understanding of the natural world which is constrained by essentialist, teleological and intentional biases. After contrasting the naturalness of religion with the unnaturalness of science, we discuss the difficulties cognitive and developmental scientists have identified in learning and accepting evolutionary theory. We indicate how religious beliefs impede students’ understanding and acceptance of evolutionary theory. We explore a number of options available to students for reconciling an informed understanding of evolutionary theory with their religious beliefs. To conclude, we discuss the implications of our account for science and biology teachers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Because we will deal here with universal aspects of the human mind, we do not have a particular religion in mind. Nevertheless, the following discussion will be particularly relevant for, and is therefore primarily intended at, learning and understanding evolutionary theory in relation to Christian beliefs.

  2. The distinction between creationist and other Christian belief systems might appear somewhat artificial in the sense that any form of Christianity introduces the belief that God somehow created this world. We will use the word creationism here to indicate the belief that God has actively and directly intervened in this world, whether in the construction of the universe, in the creation of species or the design of biological functional complexity (adaptations). Such creationist beliefs often come with a resentment against evolution. Non-creationist religious beliefs hold that God created through secondary laws, that is, indirectly and thus tend to be more science-friendly.

  3. Here, we will apply the term ‘supernatural’ as some of the authors have defined it elsewhere (Boudry et al. 2010), namely as “referring to any phenomenon which has its basis in entities and processes that transcend the spatiotemporal realm of impersonal matter and energy described by modern science.”

  4. Evans (2008), Evans et al. (2011), González Galli and Meinardi (2011), Sinatra et al. (2008).

References

  • Atran, S. (1994). Core domains versus scientific theories: Evidence from systematics and itza-maya folkbiology. In L. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 316–340). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ayala, F. J. (1970). Teleological explanations in evolutionary biology. Philosophy of Science, 37(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bardapurkar, A. (2008). Do students see the “Selection” in organic evolution? A critical review of the causal structure of student explanations. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 1, 299–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C. (2001). On the functional orgins of essentialism. Mind and Society, 2(1), 1–30. doi:10.1007/bf02512073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C. (2004). Design versus descent in Shuar children’s reasoning about animals. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(1), 25–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L. (2010). The relative unnaturalness of atheism: On why Geertz and Markusson are both right and wrong. Religion, 40(3), 169–172. doi:10.1016/j.religion.2009.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in god concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L., & Lanman, J. A. (2008). The science of religious beliefs. Religion, 38(2), 109–124. doi:10.1016/j.religion.2008.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2(3), 412–414. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bean, T. E., Sinatra, G. M., & Schrader, P. G. (2010). Spore: Spawning evolutionary misconceptions? Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19(5), 409–414. doi:10.1007/s10956-010-9211-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benassi, V. A., Singer, B., & Reynolds, C. B. (1980). Occult belief—Seeing is believing. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 19(4), 337–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, A. E., Toneatti, L., & Rosati, V. (2010). Children’s conceptions about the origin of species: A study of Italian children’s conceptions with and without instruction. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 19(4), 506–538. doi:10.1080/10508406.2010.508027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blancke, S., Boudry, M., Braeckman, J., Smedt, J. D., & Cruz, H. D. (in press). Dealing with creationist challenges. What European biology teachers might expect in the classroom. Journal of Biological Education. doi:10.1080/00219266.2010.546677.

  • Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby, how child development explains what makes us human. London: Arrow Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudry, M., Blancke, S., & Braeckman, J. (2010). How not to attack intelligent design creationism: Philosophical misconceptions about methodological naturalism. Foundations of Science, 15(3), 227–244. doi:10.1007/s10699-010-9178-7.

  • Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind, domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casler, K., & Kelemen, D. (2008). Developmental continuity in teleo-functional explanation: Reasoning about nature among Romanian Romani adults. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(3), 340–362. doi:10.1080/15248370802248556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(2), 161–199. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1402_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Buckland, L. A. (2011). Differences in epistemic practices among scientists, young earth creationists, intelligent design creationists, and the scientist-creationists of Darwin’s era. In R. S. Taylor & M. Ferrari (Eds.), Epistemology and science education. Understanding the evolution vs. Intelligent design controversy (pp. 38–76). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1986). The blind watchmaker. Harlow: Longman Scientific & Technical.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cruz, H., & De Smedt, J. (2007). The role of intuitive ontologies in scientific understanding—the case of human evolution. Biology and Philosophy, 22(3), 351–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cruz, H., & De Smedt, J. (2010). Paley’s ipod. The cognitive basis of the design argument within natural theology. Zygon. Journal of Religion and Science, 45(3), 665–684. doi:10.1111/j.1467–9744.2010.01120.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cruz, H., & De Smedt, J. (in press). Evolved cognitive biases and the epistemic status of scientific beliefs. Philosophical Studies.

  • Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C., & LaScola, L. (2010). Preachers who are not believers. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(1), 122–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diesendruck, G., & Haber, L. (2009). God’s categories: The effect of religiosity on children’s teleological and essentialist beliefs about categories. Cognition, 110(1), 100–114. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diesendruck, G., Markson, L., & Bloom, P. (2003). Children’s reliance on creator’s intent in extending names for artifacts. Psychological Science, 14(2), 164–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M. (2000a). Beyond scopes. Why creationism is here to stay. In K. Rosengren, C. Johnson, & P. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the impossible: Magical, scientific and religious thinking in children (pp. 305–331). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M. (2000b). The emergence of beliefs about the origins of species in school-age children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly-Journal of Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 221–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M. (2001). Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: Creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 42(3), 217–266. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M. (2008). Conceptual change and evolutionary biology: A developmental analysis. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research of conceptual change (pp. 263–294). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M., & Lane, J. D. (2011). Contradictory or complementary? Creationist and evolutionist explanations of the origin(s) of species. Human Development, 54(3), 144–159. doi:10.1159/000329130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, E. M., Legare, C. H., & Rosengren, K. S. (2011). Engaging multiple epistemologies. Implications for science education. In R. S. Taylor & M. Ferrari (Eds.), Epistemology and science education. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fail, J. (2008). A no-holds-barred evolution curriculum for elementary and junior high school students. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 1(1), 56–64. doi:10.1007/s12052-007-0018-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A. (2004). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 404–409. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., & Gottfried, G. M. (1994). Essentialist beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind. Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 341–365). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences. Early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38(3), 213–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Knadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • González Galli, L., & Meinardi, E. (2011). The role of teleological thinking in learning the Darwinian model of evolution. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 4(1), 145–152. doi:10.1007/s12052-010-0272-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1997). Nonoverlapping magisteria. Natural History, 106(2), 16–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, T. R. (2009). Understanding natural selection: Essential concepts and common misconceptions. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 2(2), 156–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. A new theory of religion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haught, J. F. (2000). God after Darwin: A theology of evolution. Boulder (Colo.): Westview press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J., & Wilensky, U. (2006). Complex systems in education: Scientific and educational importance and implications for the learning sciences. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(1), 11–34. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1501_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • John Paul, II. (1996). Truth cannot contradict truth. Address of Pope John Paul II to the Pontifical Academy of Sciences (October 22, 1996).

  • Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds and cognitive development. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (1999a). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70(3), 241–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (1999b). Why are rocks pointy? Children’s preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35(6), 1440–1452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (2003). British and American children’s preferences for teleo-functional explanations of the natural world. Cognition, 88(2), 201–221. doi:10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00024-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D. (2011). Teleological minds. How natural intuitions about agency and purpose influence learning about evolution. In K. S. Rosengren & E. M. Evans (Eds.), Evolution challenges: Integrating research and practice in teaching and learning about evolutionary theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Kelemen, D., Callanan, M. A., Casler, K., & Perez-Granados, D. R. (2005). Why things happen: Teleological explanation in parent-child conversations. Developmental Psychology, 41(1), 251–264. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D., & Di Yanni, C. (2005). Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent design in children’s reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(1), 3–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D., & Rosset, E. (2009). The human function compunction: Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition, 111(1), 138–143. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelemen, D., Widdowson, D., Posner, T., Brown, A. L., & Casler, K. (2003). Teleo-functional constraints on preschool children’s reasoning about living things. Developmental Science, 6(3), 329–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knight, N., Sousa, P., Barrett, J. L., & Atran, S. (2004). Children’s attributions of beliefs to humans and god: Cross-cultural evidence. Cognitive Science, 28(1), 117–126. doi:10.1016/j.cogsci.2003.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhlmeier, V. A., Bloom, P., & Wynn, K. (2004). Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects? Cognition, 94(1), 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Evans, E. M. (2010). Children’s understanding of ordinary and extraordinary minds. Child Development, 81(5), 1475–1489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, E. T. (2005). A new look at the science-and-religion dialogue. Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science, 40(3), 555–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeming, D. A., & Leeming, M. A. (1995). A dictionary of creation myths. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D., & Zaitchik, D. (2007). Inferring design—Evidence of a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Science, 18(11), 999–1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahner, M., & Bunge, M. (1996). Is religious education compatible with science education? Science & Education, 5(2), 101–123. doi:10.1007/bf00428612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Makris, N., & Pnevmatikos, D. (2007). Children’s understanding of human and super-natural mind. Cognitive Development, 22(3), 365–375. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1991). One long argument, Charles Darwin and the genesis of modern evolutionary thought. London: Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, R. N. (2000). The naturalness of religion and the unnaturalness of science. In F. C. Keil & R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 61–86). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 179–195). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. R. (1999). Finding Darwin’s god. A scientist’s search for common ground between god and evolution. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, C. E., Nickels, M. K., & Beard, J. (1998). The nature of science as foundation for teaching science: Evolution as a case study. In W. F. McComas (Ed.), The nature of science in science education. Rationales and strategies (pp. 315–328). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, G. E., Keil, F. C., Kuhlmeier, V. A., & Wynn, K. (2010). Early understandings of the link between agents and order. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(40), 17140–17145. doi:10.1073/pnas.0914056107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papineau, D. (2005). Philosophical problems of biology. In T. Honderich (Ed.), The Oxford companion to philosophy (p. 97). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennock, R. T. (1999). Tower of babel. The evidence against the new creationism. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrovich, O. (1997). Understanding of non-natural causality in children and adults: A case against artificialism. Psyche en Geloof, 8, 151–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, J., & Epley, N. (2009). Science and god: An automatic opposition between ultimate explanations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 238–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Recanati, F. (1997). Can we believe what we do not understand? Mind & Language, 12(1), 84–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Recker, D. (2010). How to confuse organisms with mousetraps: Machine metaphors and intelligent design. Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science, 45(3), 647–664. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9744.2010.01119.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruse, M. (2003). Darwin and design. Does evolution have a purpose?. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutledge, M. L., & Mitchell, M. A. (2002). High school biology teachers’ knowledge structure, acceptance, and teaching of evolution. American Biology Teacher, 64(1), 21–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sager, C. (Ed.). (2008). Voices for evolution. Berkeley: The National Center for Science Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samarapungavan, A., & Wiers, R. W. (1997). Children’s thoughts on the origin of species: A study of explanatory coherence. Cognitive Science, 21(2), 147–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Carey, S. (2005). Secret agents: Inferences about hidden causes by 10-and 12-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16(12), 995–1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, E. C. (2004). Evolution vs. Creationism: An introduction. Berkeley (Calif.): University of California press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shtulman, A. (2008). Variation in the anthropomorphization of supernatural beings and its implications for cognitive theories of religion. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 34(5), 1123–1138. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.5.1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shtulman, A., & Schulz, L. (2008). The relation between essentialist beliefs and evolutionary reasoning. Cognitive Science, 32(6), 1049–1062. doi:10.1080/03640210801897864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G. M., Brem, S., & Evans, E. (2008). Changing minds? Implications of conceptual change for teaching and learning about biological evolution. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 1(2), 189–195. doi:10.1007/s12052-008-0037-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinatra, G. M., & Nadelson, L. (2011). Science and religion. Ontologically different epistemologies? In R. S. Taylor & M. Ferrari (Eds.), Epistemology and science education. Understanding the evolution vs. Intelligent design controversy (pp. 173–193). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slone, J. (2004). Theological incorrectness. Why religious people believe what they shouldn’t. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition. A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 44–78). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture. A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P., & Findlay, S. (2010). Getting to Darwin: Obstacles to accepting evolution by natural selection. Science & Education, 19(6), 625–636. doi:10.1007/s11191-009-9204-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Ioannides, C. (1998). From conceptual development to science education: A psychological point of view. International Journal of Science Education, 20(10), 1213–1230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Vamvakoussi, X., & Skopeliti, I. (2008). The framework theory approach to the problem of conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 3–34). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Parts of this paper have been presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Religion, October 29-31, 2010, Baltimore, Maryland. This research was supported by grants BOF08/24J/041 and COM07/PWM/001 from Ghent University and by the Fund for Scientific Research Flanders (FWO). We would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their helpful remarks.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefaan Blancke.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Blancke, S., De Smedt, J., De Cruz, H. et al. The Implications of the Cognitive Sciences for the Relation Between Religion and Science Education: The Case of Evolutionary Theory. Sci & Educ 21, 1167–1184 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-011-9402-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-011-9402-z

Keywords

Navigation