Skip to main content
Log in

Parent–child interactions longitudinally mediate the association between shared literacy activities and emergent literacy and oral language skills

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research on the home literacy environment has predominantly centered on the quantity of word exposure rather than examining the role that nuanced parent–child interactions play in the home literacy environment and children’s language and literacy development. The present study addressed this gap by using the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth Cohort to examine whether the parent (e.g., sensitivity, positive regard) and child behaviors (e.g., engagement, sustained attention) during parent–child interactions assessed with the two bags task would function as mediators for the association between shared literacy activities and emergent literacy skills and oral language skills. The sample included approximately 10,690 children and their mothers. Results of the longitudinal path analysis with mediators showed the direct effects of shared literacy activities on emergent literacy and oral language outcomes. Additionally, parents’ positive regard, children’s engagement, and children’s sustained attention mediated the association between shared literacy activities and emergent literacy skills and oral language skills at preschool. Implications and future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aikens, N. L., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Socioeconomic differences in reading trajectories: The contribution of family, neighborhood, and school contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 235–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreassen, C., and Fletcher, P. (2005). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS–B), Psychometric Characteristics. Volume 1 of the ECLS-B Methodology Report for the Nine-Month Data Collection (NCES 2005–100). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

  • Baker, L., Scher, D., & Mackler, K. (1997). Home and family influences on motivations for reading. Educational Psychologist, 32(2), 69–82. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3202_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). Manual: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Gilstrap, B., & Rovine, M. (1984). The Pennsylvania Infant and Family Development Project, I: Stability and change in mother-infant and father-infant interaction in a family setting at one, three, and nine months. Child Development, 5(3), 692–705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Volume II: Separation, anxiety and anger. Attachment and Loss: Volume II: Separation, Anxiety and Anger (pp. 1–429). London: The Hogarth press and the institute of psycho-analysis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. American journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52(4), 664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical implications of attachment theory. Beijing: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., and Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon and R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Theoretical models of human development (pp. 993–1023). Wiley, NewYork

  • Burgess, S. R. (2011). Home literacy environments (HLEs) provided to very young children. Early Child Development and Care, 181(4), 445–462. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430903450384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G. (2001). Parent-child book reading through the lens of attachment theory. In L. Verhoeven & C. E. Snow (Eds.), Literacy and motivation: Reading engagement in individuals and groups (pp. 43–57). Beijing: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1988). Mother-child interactions, attachment, and emergent literacy: A cross-sectional study. Child Development, 59(5), 1262–1272. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Affective dimension of mother-infant picture book reading. Journal of School Psychology, 35(1), 47–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, B. M., and Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. University of Arkansas Little Rock.

  • Clarke, B. L. (2014). Rurality and reading readiness: The mediating role of parent engagement (R2Ed Working Paper No. 2014–1). The National Center for Research on Rural Education: r2ed.unl.edu

  • Connolly, B. H., Dalton, L., Smith, J. B., Lamberth, N. G., McCay, B., & Murphy, W. (2006). Concurrent validity of the Bayley scales of infant development II (BSID-II) motor scale and the Peabody developmental motor scale II (PDMS-2) in 12-month-old infants. Pediatric Physical Therapy, 18(3), 190–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deckner, D. F., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Child and maternal contributions to shared reading: Effects on language and literacy development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 31–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, S. E., & DeAvila, E. A. (1998). PreLAS 2000. NewYork: CTB/McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durham, R. E., Farkas, G., Hammer, C. S., Tomblin, J. B., & Catts, H. W. (2007). Kindergarten oral language skill: A key variable in the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic status. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 25(4), 294–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, D., Bagner, D. M., Pruden, S. M., & Nichols-Lopez, K. (2015). Language production in children with and at risk for delay: Mediating role of parenting skills. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44(5), 814–825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, L. G., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). The home literacy environment as a predictor of the early literacy development of children at family-risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(5), 401–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, S. R., Megens, A. M., Backman, C. L., & Hayes, V. E. (2005). Stability of the Bayley II scales of infant development in a sample of low-risk and high-risk infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47(12), 820–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmbeck, G. N., Li, S. T., Schurman, J. V., Friedman, D., & Coakley, R. M. (2002). Collecting and managing multisource and multimethod data in studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27(1), 5–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, G. E., Snow, C. E., & Porche, M. V. (2000). Project EASE: The effect of a family literacy project on kindergarten students’ early literacy skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 35(4), 524–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NewYork: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korat, O. (2009). The effect of maternal teaching talk on children’s emergent literacy as a function of type of activity and maternal education level. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.10.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korat, O., Ron, R., & Klein, P. (2008). Cognitive mediation and emotional support of fathers and mothers to their children during shared book-reading in two different SES groups. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 7(2), 223–247. https://doi.org/10.1891/194589508787381872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • La Greca, A. M., & Lemanek, K. L. (1996). Assessment as a process in pediatric psychology. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 21(2), 137–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Zucker, T., Crawford, A. D., & Solari, E. F. (2012). The effects of a responsive parenting intervention on parent–child interactions during shared book reading. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 969–986. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 3–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, D., Wong, K. K., & McBride-Chang, C. (2012). Reading motivation and reading comprehension in Chinese and English among bilingual students. Reading and Writing, 25(3), 717–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, J. A., Justice, L. M., Yumus, M., & Chaparro-Moreno, L. J. (2019). When children are not read to at home: The million word gap. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 40(5), 383–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B., White, D., McDevitt, T., & Raskin, R. (1983). Mothers’ and fathers’ speech to their young children: Similar or different? Journal of Child Language, 10(1), 245–252. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900005286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, B., & Shedden, K. (1999). Finite mixture modeling with mixture outcomes using the EM algorithm. Biometrics, 55, 463–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. (2017). Mplus user's guide: Statistical analysis with latent variables, user's guide. Muthén and Muthén.

  • Najarian, M., Snow, K., Lennon, J., & Kinsey, S. (2010). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Preschool-Kindergarten 2007 Psychometric Report (NCES 2010–009). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S: Department of Education. Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolin, M. J., Montaquila, J., Lennon, J., Kleiner, B., Kim, K., Chapman, C., Chandler, K., Creighton, S., and Bielick, S. (2000). National Household Education Survey of 1999: Data File User’s Manual, Volume I, NCES 2000–076. U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

  • Nord, C., Edwards, B., Andreassen, C., Green, J. L., & Wallner-Allen, K. (2006). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), User’s Manual for the ECLS-B Longitudinal 9-Month–2-Year Data File and Electronic Codebook (NCES 2006–046). Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics: U.S.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., Perlmutter, J. C., Galda, L., & Brody, G. H. (1990). Joint reading between black head start children and their mothers. Child Development, 61(2), 443–453. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior research methods, 40(3), 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rathmann, P. (1994). Good night, gorilla. G.P: Putnam’s Sons Books for Young Readers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L., Coker, D., & Pan, B. A. (2004). A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ talk to toddlers in low-income families. Social Development, 13(2), 278–291. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.000267.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L., & Zuckerman, B. (2016). Word gap redux: Developmental sequence and quality. JAMA pediatrics, 170(9), 827–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J., & Mackenzie, M. J. (2003). Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 613–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saracho, O. N. (2007). Fathers and young children’s literacy experiences in a family environment. Early Child Development and Care, 177(4), 403–415. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430600563034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Satorra, A. (2000). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample analysis of moment structures. In R. D. H. Heijmans, D. S. G. Pollock, & A. Satton (Eds.), Innovations in multivariare sraristical analysis: A festschrifr for Heinz Neudecker (pp. 233–247). NewYork: Kluwer Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to Grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(1), 59–87. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1001_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85, 1552–1568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445–460. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J.-A., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 96–116. https://doi.org/10.1598/rrq.33.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Son, S.-H., & Morrison, F. J. (2010). The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1103–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, D. E., Sperry, L. L., & Miller, P. J. (2019). Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development, 90(4), 1303–1318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1993). Where does knowledge come from? Specific associations between print exposure and information acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 211–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.2.211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuijl, C. V., & Leseman, P. P. (2004). Improving mother–child interaction in low-income Turkish-Dutch families: A study of mechanisms mediating improvements resulting from participating in a home-based preschool intervention program. Infant and Child Development, 13(4), 323–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, K. K., Mathews, B. L., & Kerns, K. A. (2013). Mother–child attachment and cognitive performance in middle childhood: An examination of mediating mechanisms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 259–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westerlund, M., and Lagerberg, D. (2008). Expressive vocabulary in 18‐month‐old children in relation to demographic factors, mother and child characteristics, communication style and shared reading. Child: care, health and development, 34(2), 257-266

  • Wirth, A., Ehmig, S. C., Drescher, N., Guffler, S., & Niklas, F. (2019). Facets of the early home literacy environment and children’s linguistic and socioemotional competencies. Early Education and Development, 31(6), 892–909. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2019.1706826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christine Meng.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Meng, C. Parent–child interactions longitudinally mediate the association between shared literacy activities and emergent literacy and oral language skills. Read Writ 34, 1593–1612 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10130-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10130-z

Keywords

Navigation